Refine
Year of publication
Document Type
- Article (1366)
- Postprint (163)
- Doctoral Thesis (136)
- Monograph/edited volume (97)
- Conference Proceeding (26)
- Review (25)
- Other (22)
- Preprint (20)
- Master's Thesis (3)
- Bachelor Thesis (1)
Language
Keywords
- Eye movements (25)
- eye movements (22)
- reading (20)
- Adolescence (15)
- Chinese (14)
- Reading (14)
- children (14)
- attention (13)
- aggression (12)
- Depression (10)
- adolescence (10)
- embodied cognition (10)
- Aggression (9)
- EEG (9)
- SNARC (9)
- adolescents (9)
- Blickbewegungen (8)
- Mental number line (8)
- Motivation (8)
- gender (8)
- numerical cognition (8)
- preview benefit (8)
- sexual aggression (8)
- Children (7)
- Embodied cognition (7)
- development (7)
- mental number line (7)
- middle childhood (7)
- overweight (7)
- prevention (7)
- Childhood (6)
- Cortisol (6)
- Numerical cognition (6)
- Perceptual span (6)
- SNARC effect (6)
- Stress (6)
- childhood (6)
- depression (6)
- justice sensitivity (6)
- language (6)
- longitudinal study (6)
- media violence (6)
- ADHD (5)
- Chile (5)
- Eltern (5)
- Eye movement (5)
- Eye tracking (5)
- Flow-Erleben (5)
- Heart rate variability (5)
- Intervention (5)
- Lesen (5)
- Longitudinal study (5)
- Obesity (5)
- Reading comprehension (5)
- alcohol (5)
- anxiety (5)
- dementia (5)
- motivation (5)
- obesity (5)
- parafoveal processing (5)
- parents (5)
- perceptual span (5)
- rape (5)
- social cognition (5)
- working memory (5)
- Adolescents (4)
- Attention (4)
- Body dissatisfaction (4)
- DRD4 (4)
- ERPs (4)
- Embodiment (4)
- Entwicklung (4)
- Gene-environment interaction (4)
- Interest (4)
- Kinder (4)
- Linear mixed model (4)
- Overweight (4)
- P300 (4)
- Prenatal stress (4)
- Reading development (4)
- Reading motivation (4)
- Sentence reading (4)
- Working memory (4)
- computational modeling (4)
- fixation durations (4)
- individual differences (4)
- learning (4)
- mathematics (4)
- memory (4)
- parafoveal-on-foveal effect (4)
- pornography (4)
- prevalence (4)
- quality of life (4)
- rejection sensitivity (4)
- scene viewing (4)
- self-regulation (4)
- sentence reading (4)
- victim blame (4)
- working memory capacity (4)
- working memory updating (4)
- Academic achievement (3)
- Adipositas (3)
- Alexithymia (3)
- Anxiety (3)
- Arbeit (3)
- Aufmerksamkeit (3)
- Computational modelling (3)
- Computationale Modellierung (3)
- Demenz (3)
- Dual-process (3)
- Dyslexia (3)
- Eating disorder (3)
- Emotion (3)
- Gesundheitsförderung (3)
- Infancy (3)
- Interoception (3)
- Interoceptive awareness (3)
- Jugendliche (3)
- Kognition (3)
- Lernen (3)
- Lernmotivation (3)
- Longitudinal (3)
- Mental arithmetic (3)
- Mind wandering (3)
- Operational momentum (3)
- Parafoveal processing (3)
- Psychophysiology (3)
- Sexual scripts (3)
- Turkey (3)
- Visual attention (3)
- age differences (3)
- aggressive cognitions (3)
- anger regulation (3)
- assessment (3)
- bulimia nervosa (3)
- coercive strategies (3)
- cognition (3)
- coping (3)
- diagnostic competence (3)
- eating disorders (3)
- emotion (3)
- executive function (3)
- eye movement (3)
- eye-movement control (3)
- eye-tracking (3)
- eye-voice span (3)
- fMRI (3)
- finger counting (3)
- fixation locations (3)
- inhibition (3)
- intervention (3)
- joint Simon effect (3)
- joint action (3)
- landing positions (3)
- longitudinal (3)
- narcissism (3)
- norepinephrine (3)
- numerische Kognition (3)
- operational momentum (3)
- perpetration (3)
- personality (3)
- primary school (3)
- psychology (3)
- rape myth acceptance (3)
- relationship constellations (3)
- robbery (3)
- saccades (3)
- scene perception (3)
- self-efficacy (3)
- sentence processing (3)
- sexual scripts (3)
- social support (3)
- spatial frequencies (3)
- strain (3)
- stress (3)
- synchronization (3)
- victimization (3)
- visual attention (3)
- work (3)
- workplace (3)
- Übergewicht (3)
- Adolescent (2)
- Age at First Drink (2)
- Alcohol dependence (2)
- Alzheimer (2)
- Angewandte Sozialpsychologie (2)
- Anorexia nervosa (2)
- Arbeitsgedächtnis (2)
- Arbeitsgestaltung (2)
- Attribution (2)
- Augenbewegungen (2)
- Autonomic response (2)
- Beanspruchung (2)
- Behavior therapy (2)
- Binge eating (2)
- Brazil (2)
- Bulimia nervosa (2)
- Childhood adversity (2)
- Chinese reading (2)
- Cognitive control (2)
- Cognitive-behavioral therapy (2)
- Coping (2)
- DLT (2)
- Development (2)
- Dysregulation (2)
- ERP (2)
- Early psychosocial adversity (2)
- Elite athlete (2)
- Emotion recognition (2)
- Emotional intelligence (2)
- Emotions (2)
- English (2)
- Ereigniskorreliertes Potenzial (2)
- Ernährung (2)
- Essstörung (2)
- European Union (2)
- Event-related potential (2)
- Event-related potentials (2)
- Externalizing behavior (2)
- FAP (2)
- FKBP5 (2)
- Facial expression (2)
- Fixational eye movements (2)
- German (2)
- Germany (2)
- HPA (2)
- HPA axis (2)
- Human (2)
- IAT (2)
- Imitation (2)
- Interoceptive sensitivity (2)
- Jugendalter (2)
- Justice sensitivity (2)
- Learning (2)
- Lehrer (2)
- Lehrergesundheit (2)
- Lernstrategien (2)
- MAOA (2)
- Media violence (2)
- Memory (2)
- Mental disorder (2)
- Microsaccade (2)
- Middle childhood (2)
- Mother-infant interaction (2)
- Moving window (2)
- Narcissism (2)
- Neurofeedback (2)
- Oculomotor control (2)
- Parafoveal (2)
- Personality (2)
- Persönlichkeit (2)
- Poland (2)
- Preschoolers (2)
- Prävention (2)
- Psychosoziale Gesundheit (2)
- Retrieval (2)
- Saccade (2)
- Self-efficacy (2)
- Self-esteem (2)
- Semantic (2)
- Sentence comprehension (2)
- Sexual aggression (2)
- Sexual victimization (2)
- Short-term memory (2)
- Somatic (2)
- Somatoform disorder (2)
- Spanish (2)
- Spatial bias (2)
- Spatial-numerical associations (2)
- Sport (2)
- Text difficulty (2)
- Theory of Mind (2)
- To learners in which of the following categories does your work apply (2)
- Türkei (2)
- Verbal cues (2)
- Visual search (2)
- Young adulthood (2)
- abstract concepts (2)
- acculturation (2)
- achievement (2)
- action observation (2)
- action words (2)
- activation (2)
- aggressive behavior (2)
- aging (2)
- anorexia nervosa (2)
- anticipation (2)
- antilocality (2)
- arithmetic (2)
- associative networks (2)
- body dissatisfaction (2)
- calculation (2)
- childhood abuse (2)
- children and adolescents (2)
- children obesity (2)
- chronic illness (2)
- cognitive-control (2)
- compensation strategies (2)
- computer-based training (2)
- conditional reasoning (2)
- conduct problems (2)
- consistency (2)
- construct validity (2)
- criminology (2)
- decision-theory (2)
- depressive symptoms (2)
- developmental dyscalculia (2)
- disordered eating (2)
- disturbed eating (2)
- dynamical model (2)
- dysfunktionale Einstellungen (2)
- dyslexia (2)
- e-z reader (2)
- eating behavior (2)
- eating disorder (2)
- emotion recognition (2)
- emotional expression (2)
- emotions (2)
- empathy (2)
- evaluative study (2)
- event-related potentials (2)
- exercise (2)
- expectation (2)
- eye tracking (2)
- face morphing (2)
- facial feedback (2)
- family relations (2)
- gaze-contingent displays (2)
- gene-environment interaction (2)
- gestörtes Essverhalten (2)
- health promotion (2)
- hostile attribution bias (2)
- human behaviour (2)
- hypochondriasis (2)
- ideal-observer model (2)
- impulsivity (2)
- infant (2)
- interference model (2)
- job characteristics (2)
- kindliche Adipositas (2)
- leniency bias (2)
- linear mixed model (2)
- locality (2)
- locus coeruleus (2)
- mass media (2)
- mathematics instruction (2)
- mental arithmetic (2)
- mental simulation (2)
- mentale Zahlenrepräsentation (2)
- mislocated fixations (2)
- mittlere Kindheit (2)
- modeling (2)
- moral disgust sensitivity (2)
- multilevel analysis (2)
- neuropsychology (2)
- numerical development (2)
- object-based attention (2)
- parafoveal (2)
- parafoveal preview benefit (2)
- parafoveal vision (2)
- perceived stress (2)
- personality change (2)
- phonological awareness (2)
- physical activity (2)
- plasticity (2)
- prediction (2)
- proactive work behavior (2)
- prosocial behavior (2)
- provocation sensitivity (2)
- psychologinguistics (2)
- psychometric properties (2)
- psychotherapy (2)
- pubertal timing (2)
- puberty (2)
- rampage (2)
- random number generation (2)
- reading motivation (2)
- reciprocal relationship (2)
- relational aggression (2)
- religiosity (2)
- reproductive strategies (2)
- risk factor (2)
- saccade generation (2)
- salivary alpha-amylase (2)
- school shooting (2)
- semantic (2)
- sentence comprehension (2)
- sex differences (2)
- sexual assault (2)
- sexual victimization (2)
- sexual violence (2)
- sick leave (2)
- similarity (2)
- social rejection (2)
- soziale Beziehungen (2)
- spatial attention (2)
- spatial cognition (2)
- spatial statistics (2)
- stimulus-response compatibility (2)
- tVNS (2)
- theory of mind (2)
- token resistance (2)
- trait anger (2)
- tunnel vision (2)
- unipolar affective disorders (2)
- validation (2)
- visual perception (2)
- visual search (2)
- well-being (2)
- young adults (2)
- young athletes (2)
- Ärgerregulation (2)
- 13-to 15-month-old infants (1)
- 2AFC (1)
- 46 (3) 2009 (1)
- 5-HTTLPR (1)
- ACTH (1)
- ADHS (1)
- AHAB (1)
- ASI-3 (1)
- Abdominal pain (1)
- Abhängigkeitsauflösung (1)
- Acetylcholin (1)
- Achievement (1)
- Achilles and patellar tendon (1)
- Action (1)
- Action prediction (1)
- Action processing (1)
- Action representation (1)
- Action simulation (1)
- Action-perception (1)
- Activation suppression model (1)
- Active avoidance (1)
- Active vision (1)
- Adaptation (1)
- Additive and interactive effects (1)
- Adverbial Quantification (1)
- Adverbs of Frequency (1)
- Adverbs of Quantity (1)
- Affect (1)
- Affekt (1)
- Affordance (1)
- Ageing (1)
- Agency (1)
- Aggression Replacement Training (1)
- Aktualisierung (1)
- Alan Kennedy (1)
- Alcohol Use (1)
- Allgemeine Psychologie (1)
- Alpha-linolenic acid (1)
- Alter (1)
- Altersunterschiede (1)
- Ambiguity (1)
- American Indians (1)
- Amygdala (1)
- Anger control (1)
- Anger regulation (1)
- Angst (1)
- Angsterkrankung (1)
- Animacy (1)
- Anreize (1)
- Anticipatory gaze shifts (1)
- Antidepressiva (1)
- Antipsychotika (1)
- Anxiety Sensitivity Index (1)
- Aphasia (1)
- Applied Psychology (1)
- Approach and avoidance behavior (1)
- Arbeitsgedächtniskapazität (1)
- Arbeitsmotivation (1)
- Arbeitsplatz (1)
- Arbeitsplatzphobie (1)
- Arbeitsunfähigkeit (1)
- Arousal (1)
- Articulation duration (1)
- Athlet (1)
- Attention deficit (1)
- Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (1)
- Attention: Selective (1)
- Attitude (1)
- Attitudes towards inclusion (1)
- Attraction (1)
- Attractiveness (1)
- Attrition (1)
- Auditory pitch (1)
- Aufgabenwechsel (1)
- Aufmerksamkeitskontrolle (1)
- Autokorrelationen (1)
- Automatizität (1)
- Automobildesign (1)
- Autonomic (1)
- Autonomic activity (1)
- Aversionsgefühle (1)
- BDNF (1)
- BIA model (1)
- BIQUA (1)
- BMI (1)
- BMI development (1)
- Background texture (1)
- Bayesian brain (1)
- Bayesian estimation (1)
- Bayesian inference (1)
- Bayesian modeling (1)
- Bedeutung (1)
- Befundintegration (1)
- Belastung (1)
- Berufseinstieg (1)
- Beverages (1)
- Bewegung (1)
- Bewältigungsstrategien (1)
- Bezugsnorm-Orientierung (1)
- Bildgeschichtenaufgabe (1)
- Bildungsqualität (1)
- Bilinear models (1)
- Biofeedback (1)
- Biological motion (1)
- Blickbewegung (1)
- Blickbewegungen beim Lesen (1)
- Blickbewegungskontrolle (1)
- Blickspuren (1)
- Block copolymer (1)
- Body image (1)
- Body size perception (1)
- Body stimuli (1)
- Bodybuilding (1)
- Bottom-up (1)
- Boundary paradigm (1)
- Boundary technique (1)
- Boys (1)
- Brain potentials (1)
- Breastfeeding (1)
- Brinley analysis (1)
- Brinley plot (1)
- Brinley-Analyse (1)
- Brinley-Plot (1)
- Burn-out-Syndrom (1)
- CAR (1)
- CBCL (1)
- CELEX (1)
- CNR1 (1)
- CNV (1)
- Cantonese (1)
- Capacities (1)
- Capacity (1)
- Capacity disorders (1)
- Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene (1)
- Category effect (1)
- Category identification (1)
- Change Management (1)
- Child (1)
- Child language (1)
- Child's emotional eating (1)
- Childhood obesity (1)
- Chinese characters (1)
- Choice deferral (1)
- Chronic abdominal pain (1)
- Chronic stress (1)
- Chronische Schmerzen (1)
- Cigarette smoke (1)
- Classification and diagnostics (1)
- Classroom-level effects (1)
- Clinical judgment (1)
- Cloze probability (1)
- Cognitive behavioral intervention (1)
- Cognitive eye movements (1)
- Cognitive therapy (1)
- Cognitive tropism (1)
- Cognitive-behavioral treatment (1)
- Colitis ulcerosa (1)
- Color vision Aging (1)
- Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR) (1)
- Common Sense Model of Illness Representation (1)
- Common coding (1)
- Communication (1)
- Communion (1)
- Comorbidity (1)
- Company success (1)
- Complementary feeding (1)
- Complementary food (1)
- Comprehension (1)
- Computational modeling (1)
- Computational psychiatry (1)
- Computer Augmented Reality (1)
- Computer-assisted self-regulation training (1)
- Computernutzung (1)
- Conduct problems (1)
- Connectivity (1)
- Consciousness (1)
- Consumer choice (1)
- Consumption (1)
- Context-specific task features (1)
- Continuous performance task (1)
- Corpus (1)
- Corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1 gene (1)
- Cortisone (1)
- Costs (1)
- Counting (1)
- Creaminess (1)
- Crohn's disease (1)
- Cross-cultural (1)
- Cultural intelligence (1)
- Current motivation (1)
- Current source density reconstruction (1)
- Cutaneous pain perception (1)
- DHA (1)
- DHA status (1)
- DHA supplements (1)
- Daily Hassles (1)
- Dark side (1)
- Data transformation (1)
- Deaf readers (1)
- Death-threats (1)
- Decision making (1)
- Decision-making (1)
- Decoupling (1)
- Decoupling hypothesis (1)
- Deduction (1)
- Defensive response patterns (1)
- Defocused attention (1)
- Dekodierung (1)
- Delta plot (1)
- Depressive symptoms (1)
- Determinants (1)
- Deutschland (1)
- Development of eating behavior (1)
- Diagnostic skills (1)
- Diagnostik (1)
- Disability (1)
- Discourse-linking (1)
- Discriminative learning (1)
- Disordered eating (1)
- Disordered eating behaviours (1)
- Distanzierung (1)
- Distributed processing (1)
- Distributional analyses (1)
- Disturbances of embodiment (1)
- Divergent thinking (1)
- Diversity (1)
- Domain specificity (1)
- Domain-specific knowledge (1)
- Dopamine (1)
- Doping (1)
- Doping attitude (1)
- Drinking Behavior (1)
- Dual task (1)
- Dual-pathway model (1)
- Dual-task interference (1)
- Dyslexia assessment (1)
- Dyslexic children (1)
- Dysphoria (1)
- ECG (1)
- EDA (1)
- EEG/ERP (1)
- EKG (1)
- EMG (1)
- EMG biofeedback (1)
- EMG-Biofeedback (1)
- EMG-biofeedback (1)
- Early adolescence (1)
- Early adversity (1)
- Early maternal care (1)
- Early social cognition (1)
- Eating (1)
- Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (1)
- Eating disorders (1)
- Eating pathology (1)
- Education, patients (1)
- Effects of trial history (1)
- Effort discounting (1)
- Elementary school students (1)
- Eltern-Kind-Assoziation (1)
- Embodied perception (1)
- Emotion Regulation (1)
- Emotion labelling (1)
- Emotion regulation (1)
- Emotional expressions (1)
- Emotionale Erschöpfung (1)
- Emotionsregulation (1)
- Empowerment (1)
- Endogenous attention (1)
- Energy metabolism (1)
- English as a foreign language (1)
- Entfremdung (1)
- Entrainment (1)
- Entrepreneurs (1)
- Epigenetic (1)
- Epigenetics (1)
- Error-correction (1)
- Erzählendes Schreiben (1)
- Essstörungen (1)
- Eudämonie (1)
- Europe (1)
- Eurythmy therapy (1)
- Evaluation (1)
- Event related potential (1)
- Event-related potentials (ERPs) (1)
- Evoked potentials (1)
- Evolutionspsychologie (1)
- Exclusion of alternatives (1)
- Exclusiveness (1)
- Exercise (1)
- Expected satiation (1)
- Expected satiety (1)
- Expected thirst (1)
- Experience Sampling Method (ESM) (1)
- Experiment (1)
- Experimental design (1)
- Experimental evaluation (1)
- Experimentalverlauf (1)
- Expertise (1)
- Externalizing disorders (1)
- Eye movements during reading (1)
- Eye movements in reading (1)
- Eye-fixation-related potentials (EFRP) (1)
- Eyemind assumption (1)
- Face categorization (1)
- Fachwissen (1)
- Facial recognition (1)
- Family adversity (1)
- Fast-priming (1)
- Feeding practices (1)
- Fehlbeanspruchungen (1)
- Femininität (1)
- Figursorgen (1)
- Finger counting habits (1)
- Fingerzählen (1)
- Fish (1)
- Fixational selectivity (1)
- Fixationdauern (1)
- Fixationspositionen (1)
- Fixationssequenzen (1)
- Flow (1)
- Flow-Erlebnis (1)
- Flow-Experience (1)
- Flow-Kurz-Skala (1)
- Flow-Kurzskala (FKS) (1)
- Flow-experience (1)
- Fludrocortisone (1)
- Focus (1)
- Folteropfer (1)
- Font size (1)
- Food approach (1)
- Food avoidance (1)
- Food deprivation (1)
- Foveal load hypothesis (1)
- Freezing (1)
- Freizeit (1)
- Freizeitmotivation (1)
- Frequency (1)
- Frequenz (1)
- Frontopolar (1)
- Frühdiagnose (1)
- Funktionelle Bauchschmerzen (1)
- GHQ (1)
- GIRK2 (1)
- Gaze-contingent displays (1)
- Gedankenschweifen (1)
- Gedankenverlorenes Lesen (1)
- Gedächtnis (1)
- Gedächtnisambulanz (1)
- Gender Identity (1)
- Gender Stereotypes (1)
- Gender differences (1)
- Gender role stereotypes (1)
- General practice (1)
- Genetic association (1)
- German version (1)
- Gerontologie (1)
- Geschlechterstereotype (1)
- Geschlechtsidentität (1)
- Geschlechtsrollenselbstkonzept (1)
- Geschmackssystems (1)
- Gesundheit (1)
- Gesundheitsressourcen (1)
- Gewichtssorgen (1)
- Ghrelin (1)
- Girls (1)
- Glaubwürdigkeit (1)
- Glucocorticoid receptor (1)
- Glück/Zufriedenheit (1)
- Goal salience (1)
- Goal saliency (1)
- Goal-directed movements (1)
- Grades (1)
- Grasp affordances (1)
- Grounded cognition (1)
- Groundedness (1)
- Grundschule (1)
- Grundschüler (1)
- Guidelines (1)
- GxE interaction (1)
- HIV (1)
- Hacker (1)
- Hair cortisol (1)
- Half-longitudinal (1)
- Hand kinematics (1)
- Handlungsregulation (1)
- Haplotype (1)
- Haptics (1)
- Health related quality of life (1)
- Health-related quality of life (1)
- Healthy eating behavior (1)
- Heartbeat perception (1)
- Height (1)
- Height SOS changes (1)
- Hemmung (1)
- Herzrate (1)
- Herzratenvariabilität (1)
- Heuristik (1)
- Hindi (1)
- Hirnentwicklung (1)
- Historical trend (1)
- Hochleistungssport (1)
- Hot/cool executive function (1)
- Human Resources Management (1)
- Human service work (1)
- Human sexuality (1)
- Hunger (1)
- Hypothesis Test (1)
- Hypothetical rewards (1)
- IC model (1)
- ICF (1)
- IHE attack (1)
- IOVP effect (1)
- IOVP-Effekt (1)
- Identity Structure Analysis (1)
- Implicit association test (IAT) (1)
- Implicit attitude test (IAT) (1)
- Impulsivity (1)
- Impulsivität (1)
- Indirect test (1)
- Individual differences (1)
- Infant (1)
- Infant action processing (1)
- Infant regulatory problems (1)
- Infants (1)
- Infants (age: 7 months) (1)
- Information Processing (1)
- Information form (1)
- Inhibition of return (1)
- Initiation (1)
- Innovativität (1)
- Instandhaltung (1)
- Instructional practices (1)
- Insula (1)
- Intelligenz (1)
- Interaction (1)
- Interactive activation model (1)
- Intercultural competence (1)
- Intercultural friendships (1)
- Interesse (1)
- Internal simulation (1)
- Internalizing behavior (1)
- Internalizing symptoms (1)
- International collaboration (1)
- Interpersonal Reactivity Index (1)
- Intertrial coherence (1)
- Intra-individual response-time variability (1)
- Intra-parietal sulcus (1)
- Intrauterine exposure (1)
- Intuitive eating scale (1)
- Iranian Plateau (1)
- Job-anxiety (1)
- Junge Mutter / Kind / Aggressivität / Risikoverhalten (1)
- KCNJ6 (1)
- Kausalität (1)
- Kinder und Jugendliche (1)
- Kindergarten (1)
- Kinderpsychologie / Jugendpsychologie / Psychische Störung (1)
- Kindertagesstätten (1)
- Kindheit (1)
- Kinesthetic representations (1)
- Klassifikationsgüte (1)
- Kleinkinder (1)
- Klinisch-psychologischer Test (1)
- Klinische Psychologie (1)
- Kognitive Motivation (1)
- Kompetenzentwicklung (1)
- Komponentenanalyse (1)
- Konditional (1)
- Konditionalregeln (1)
- Konditionalsatz (1)
- Konferenz (1)
- Konsistenz (1)
- Kontingenzansatz (1)
- Korean (1)
- Korean readers (1)
- Koregistrierung (1)
- Korrektursakkaden (1)
- Krankheitsbewältigung (1)
- Kriegsopfer (1)
- Körperunzufriedenheit (1)
- LCST (1)
- LIST (1)
- LIWC (1)
- Landepositionen (1)
- Landepositionsfehler (1)
- Landing position (1)
- Language (1)
- Language development (1)
- Late positive potential (1)
- Latent-state-trait modeling (1)
- Leadership (1)
- Leaking (1)
- Learning motivation (1)
- Lebensqualität (1)
- Lehrertraining (1)
- Leistung (1)
- Leistungsmotivation (1)
- Lerntechnik (1)
- Lernverlauf (1)
- Lese-Rechtschreibstörung (1)
- Leseförderung (1)
- Lesestrategie (1)
- Levels of processing (1)
- Levels-of-inattention Hypothese (1)
- Lexical database (1)
- Lexical decision (1)
- Lexical tone (1)
- Libet (1)
- Linear mixed models (1)
- Logisches Denken (1)
- Long-term memory (1)
- Longitudinal Study (1)
- Low frequency amplitude variability (1)
- Längsschnitt (1)
- Längsschnittstudie (1)
- MASC (1)
- MBI (1)
- MDS (1)
- Macular pigment (1)
- Magnitude comparison (1)
- Magnitude processing (1)
- MaiStep (1)
- Maintenance (1)
- Maltreatment (1)
- Mannheim Study of Children at Risk (1)
- Mannheimer Risikokinderstudie (1)
- Maskulinität (1)
- Mastery goals (1)
- Maternal perception (1)
- Maternal weight (1)
- Mathematical model (1)
- Mathematik (1)
- Mathematikunterricht (1)
- Media Psychology (1)
- Media Violence (1)
- Mediengewalt (1)
- Medienpsychologie (1)
- Medizinstudium (1)
- Mehrebenenanalyse (1)
- Memory-guided saccades (1)
- Mental Health (1)
- Mental Models Theory (1)
- Mental Number (1)
- Mental disease (1)
- Mental disorders (1)
- Mental health (1)
- Mental speed (1)
- Mentale Modell Theorie (1)
- Meta-analysis (1)
- Metacognitive strategy knowledge (1)
- Metakognition (1)
- Metaphor (1)
- Methode der Orte (1)
- Methodology (1)
- Methylation (1)
- Metrum (1)
- Microsaccades (1)
- Microsaccadic Inhibition (1)
- Mikrosakkaden (1)
- Mind-body-therapy (1)
- Mindfulness (1)
- Mineralocorticoid receptor (1)
- Mini-ICF-APP (1)
- MoMo-AFB (1)
- MoVo (1)
- Modality (1)
- Modalitätspaarungen (1)
- Model linkage (1)
- Model of Analogue and Symbolic Codes (1)
- Modell der Bayesianischen Sakkadenplanung (1)
- Modelling (1)
- Modelllernen (1)
- Molecular heterosis (1)
- Moral reasoning (1)
- Morbus Crohn (1)
- Morbus Parkinson (1)
- Morphological structure (1)
- Mother-Child Interaction (1)
- Mother-infant behavior (1)
- Motivation <Psychologie> (1)
- Motive (1)
- Motor control (1)
- Motor execution (1)
- Motor resonance account (1)
- Moving window paradigm (1)
- Musik (1)
- Musikrhythmus (1)
- Muskelsorgen (1)
- Mutter-Kind Interaktion (1)
- Mutter-Kind-Interaktion (1)
- Mütter (1)
- N+2-boundary paradigm (1)
- N170 (1)
- N2 (1)
- N400 (1)
- Nachwuchssport (1)
- Nahrungsergänzungsmittel (1)
- Naming (1)
- Narcissistic leadership (1)
- Nc (1)
- Need for action (1)
- Negative Emotionality (1)
- Negative Emotionalität (1)
- Neglect (1)
- Neoplasms, psychology (1)
- Neuropeptide Y (1)
- Neuroscience (1)
- Neutral tone (1)
- Nicotin (1)
- Nicotine (1)
- Nonlinear mixed-effects modeling (1)
- Norepinephrine transporter (1)
- Nucleus parabrachialis (1)
- Number cognition (1)
- Number processing (1)
- Number representation (1)
- Number-space association (1)
- Numerical cognaion (1)
- Numerical distance effect (1)
- Numerical estimation (1)
- Numerical magnitude (1)
- Nutrients (1)
- OCD (1)
- Obesity risk (1)
- Object categorization (1)
- Occupational stress (1)
- Occupational therapists (1)
- Ocular drift (1)
- Oculomotor resonance (1)
- Okulomotorik (1)
- Old/New effect (1)
- Operand order effect (1)
- Optimale und Präferierte Blickposition (1)
- Optimism (1)
- Orbitofrontal (1)
- Organisationsentwicklung (1)
- Organisationswandel (1)
- Ostensive communication (1)
- P2 (1)
- P3 (1)
- P30 0 (1)
- P300Psychophysiology (1)
- PMd (1)
- PTSD (1)
- Pain (1)
- Pain threshold (1)
- Pain tolerance (1)
- Parafoveal vision (1)
- Parafoveal-on-foveal effects (1)
- Parallele Verarbeitung (1)
- Parental pressure (1)
- Parenting practices (1)
- Parenting quality (1)
- Parents (1)
- Parsing difficulty (1)
- Part of speech (1)
- Partizipationsstörung (1)
- Pathological Narcissism Inventory (1)
- Patientenschulung (1)
- Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer (1)
- Peer pressure (1)
- Peer-Einflüsse (1)
- Peers (1)
- Perceived stress (1)
- Perception (1)
- Perception and action (1)
- Perception-action-link (1)
- Performance setting (1)
- Person-centered approach (1)
- Personal initiative (1)
- Personality traits (1)
- Personenbezogene Dienstleistungstätigkeiten (1)
- Perspective taking (1)
- Persönlichkeitsentwicklung (1)
- Pflegeberuf (1)
- Phase Synchronization (1)
- Phonological (1)
- Physical activity (1)
- Physical aggression (1)
- Physical exercise (1)
- Physicians (1)
- Physikunterricht (1)
- Physiologische Psychologie (1)
- Placebo (1)
- Plastizität (1)
- PoNeGri (1)
- Point-light action (1)
- Pointing (1)
- Polymorphism (1)
- Posner cueing (1)
- Postpartale Depression (1)
- Postpartum anxiety disorders (1)
- Postpartum depression (1)
- Potentially real rewards (1)
- Potsdamer-Motivations-Inventar (PMI) (1)
- Pragmatic inference (1)
- Pre-school children (1)
- Predictability (1)
- Prediction (1)
- Predictive coding (1)
- Predictors (1)
- Preference for solitude (1)
- Pregnancy (1)
- Preterm infant (1)
- Prevalence (1)
- Prevention (1)
- Preview (1)
- Preview Benefit (1)
- Preview benefit (1)
- Preview effects (1)
- Prior knowledge (1)
- Probabilistische Theorie (1)
- Problematic cannabis use (1)
- Processing speed (1)
- Produkterleben (1)
- Programming library (1)
- Progredienzangst (1)
- Pronominal anaphora (1)
- Pronouns (1)
- Prosocial behaviour (1)
- Prospective Longitudinal Study (1)
- Prospective study (1)
- Protective factor (1)
- Prävalenz (1)
- Pseudocleft (1)
- Psychische Belastung (1)
- Psychische Gesundheit (1)
- Psycholinguistik (1)
- Psychological publications (1)
- Psychologie (1)
- Psychologiestudium (1)
- Psychometric properties (1)
- Psychopathology (1)
- Psychophysiologie (1)
- Psychosocial functioning (1)
- Psychosocial health risks and resources (1)
- Psychosocial stress (1)
- Puberty (1)
- Python (1)
- Pädagogik (1)
- Quality of life (1)
- Questionnaire (1)
- Questionnaires (1)
- RAFT polymerization (1)
- RNG (1)
- ROC curve (1)
- RT models (1)
- Ramsey test (1)
- Ramsey-Test (1)
- Randomized controlled trial (1)
- Randomized-controlled trial (1)
- Rapeseed oil (1)
- Rapid automatized naming (1)
- Rational action understanding (1)
- Rational imitation tasks (1)
- Raub (1)
- Reading Strategy (1)
- Reading direction (1)
- Reading strategy (1)
- Real-time prediction (1)
- Reappraisal (1)
- Reasoning (1)
- Reasoning ability (1)
- Rechnen (1)
- Rechtspsychologie (1)
- Reciprocal Teaching (1)
- Recognition memory (1)
- Red (1)
- Reduktion (1)
- Reference (1)
- Referent Selection (1)
- Regressionsanalysen (1)
- Regressionsmodell (1)
- Rehearsal (1)
- Reinforcement learning (1)
- Reiz-Reaktions-Zuordnung (1)
- Rejection sensitivity (1)
- Relational aggression (1)
- Relative dose response test (1)
- Relaxation (1)
- Reliability (1)
- Religiosity (1)
- Religiosität (1)
- Resilience (1)
- Response (1)
- Responsive polymer (1)
- Restraint eating (1)
- Review (1)
- Reziprokes Lehren (1)
- Risiko (1)
- Risiko- und Vulnerabilitätsfaktoren (1)
- Risikofaktoren (1)
- Risk and Vulnerability Factors (1)
- Russian (1)
- S-R Kompatibilität (1)
- S-R compatibility (1)
- S. 635-644 (1)
- SCPs (1)
- SD3 (1)
- SFON (1)
- SMARC (1)
- SNARC Effekt (1)
- Saccade latency (1)
- Saccade planning (1)
- Saccade-target selection (1)
- Saccadic error (1)
- Saccadic facilitation effect (1)
- Sakkade (1)
- Saliency (1)
- Satzverarbeitung (1)
- Scene perception (1)
- Scene viewing (1)
- Schizophrenic self-assembly (1)
- Schlaf- und Traumforschung (1)
- Schlussfolgern (1)
- School principals (1)
- Schreibfertigkeiten (1)
- Schreibstrategien (1)
- Schulbindung (1)
- Schulen (1)
- Schulleistung (1)
- Schulleitung (1)
- Schüler (1)
- Secondary saccade (1)
- Secondary school children (1)
- Sekundarstufe 1 (1)
- Sekundärsakkaden (1)
- Selbstbestimmungstheorie (1)
- Selbstgesteuertes Lernen (1)
- Selbstmanagement (1)
- Selbstwirksamkeit (1)
- Selbstzuschreibung (1)
- Selection processes (1)
- Selective attention (1)
- Self (1)
- Self-assessment manikin (SAM) (1)
- Self-regulation (1)
- Semantic preview benefit (1)
- Semantik (1)
- Sensory cues (1)
- Sequential instruction (1)
- Serial and parallel (1)
- Serial order memory (1)
- Serial recall (1)
- Sexual Aggression (1)
- Sexual Orientation (1)
- Sexuality (1)
- Sexuelle Aggression (1)
- Short dark triad (1)
- Short term growth (1)
- Shyness (1)
- Sick Leave (1)
- Sign language (1)
- Signal detection theory (1)
- Simon effect (1)
- Single-blind (1)
- Situated cognition (1)
- Situatedness (1)
- Situations (1)
- Situationsansatz (1)
- Situative Anforderungen (1)
- Skalenaufbau (1)
- Skin conductance (1)
- Skipping (1)
- Slow positive wave (1)
- Smoking during pregnancy (1)
- Social Identity Theory (1)
- Social Psychology (1)
- Social cognition (1)
- Social cues (1)
- Social learning (1)
- Social performance (1)
- Social rejection (1)
- Social relationships (1)
- Social skills (1)
- Social stress (1)
- Socialization (1)
- Software (1)
- Southeast Asia (1)
- Soziale Unterstützung (1)
- Soziale Verantwortung (1)
- Soziale Wahrnehmung (1)
- Sozialer Druck (1)
- Sozialpsychologie (1)
- Spatial attention (1)
- Spatial coding (1)
- Spatial cognition (1)
- Spatial frequencies (1)
- Spatial memory (1)
- Spatial-numerical association of response codes (1)
- Spatial-numerical association of response codes effect (1)
- Speech perception (1)
- Spontaneous memory (1)
- Sportler (1)
- Sprachdiagnostik (1)
- Sprache (1)
- Spracherwerb (1)
- Sprachrhythmus (1)
- Stabilization (1)
- Standard Indonesian (1)
- Statistical learning (1)
- Stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) (1)
- Stimulus presentation (1)
- Stimulus-Onset Asynchrony (1)
- Strategie (1)
- Strategietraining (1)
- Strategy Training (1)
- Stress inoculation (1)
- Stresserleben (1)
- Stressful Life Events (1)
- Stroop (1)
- Student motivation (1)
- Success (1)
- Suicide (1)
- Sulfobetaine methacrylate (1)
- Superior colliculus (1)
- Supervisee Levels Questionnaire (1)
- Supervision (1)
- Surprisal (1)
- Surrogate Data (1)
- Sustained attention (1)
- Syllabic tone (1)
- Sympathovagal balance (1)
- Symposium (1)
- Syntax (1)
- Szenenbetrachtung (1)
- Szenenwahrnehmung (1)
- Tagung (1)
- Target eccentricity (1)
- Teacher motivation (1)
- Teachers (1)
- Team Composition (1)
- Team Development (1)
- Temperament (1)
- Tense (1)
- Tense Semantics (1)
- Test anxiety (1)
- Testing the limits (1)
- Text orientation (1)
- Theorie der Zielsysteme (1)
- Time perception (1)
- Time reference (1)
- Top-down (1)
- Top-down influences (1)
- Topographic consistency (1)
- Training conditions (1)
- Trainingsevaluation (1)
- Transparent orthography (1)
- Transsexuality (1)
- Transsexualität (1)
- Turkish German (1)
- UCST (1)
- Uighur (1)
- Umschriebene Entwicklungsstörung (1)
- Unternehmensentwicklung / Management (1)
- Unternehmenserfolg (1)
- Unterricht (1)
- Urteilsfindung (1)
- Urteilsgenauigkeit (1)
- Usability (1)
- Valence (1)
- Validity (1)
- Verantwortungszuschreibung (1)
- Verbal memory (1)
- Verbs (1)
- Verfahrensvalidierung (1)
- Vergewaltigung (1)
- Vergewaltigungsmythen (1)
- Verhaltensprobleme (1)
- Veränderungsprinzipien (1)
- Vision (1)
- Visual Oddball Paradigm (1)
- Visual representations (1)
- Visual scanpath (1)
- Visual stimulus processing (1)
- Visual working memory (1)
- Visually-guided saccades (1)
- Visuelle Aufmerksamkeit (1)
- Vitamin A (1)
- Vorhersagbarkeit (1)
- Vorurteil (1)
- Voxel-based morphometry (1)
- Wahrnehmung (1)
- Walking (1)
- Weight (1)
- Weight and muscle concerns (1)
- Weight loss (1)
- Weight management trial (1)
- Weight stigma (1)
- Werte (1)
- Werteerziehung (1)
- Werteprioritäten (1)
- Wille (1)
- Wissen (1)
- Wissensdiagnose (1)
- Wohlbefinden (1)
- Word boundaries (1)
- Word form area (1)
- Word frequency (1)
- Word learning (1)
- Word order (1)
- Word reading (1)
- Word recognition (1)
- Work ability (1)
- Workload (1)
- Workplace (1)
- Wortassoziationsversuch (1)
- Wortgrenzen (1)
- Yale Food Addiction Scale (1)
- Young Adulthood (1)
- Young adults (1)
- Zeitdruck (1)
- Ziele (1)
- Zieleffektivitätstraining (1)
- Zielorientierung (1)
- Zoom lens model of attention (1)
- Zwei-Prozess (1)
- Zählen (1)
- abdominal pain (1)
- academic achievement (1)
- academic failure (1)
- academic self-concept (1)
- action perception (1)
- action planning (1)
- action processing (1)
- action understanding (1)
- actions (1)
- actylcholine (1)
- additive and interactive effects (1)
- adherence (1)
- adolescent (1)
- adolescent immigrants (1)
- adolescent sexuality (1)
- adult age differences (1)
- aesthetic preferences (1)
- aesthetic user requirements (1)
- affect (1)
- affective priming (1)
- age (1)
- age at first cigarette (1)
- agency (1)
- aggressive norms (1)
- alcohol use (1)
- alcohol use disorder (1)
- alcoholism (1)
- alienation (1)
- ambiguity (1)
- amygdala (1)
- animations (1)
- antisocial (1)
- anxiety disorder (1)
- anxiety sensitivity (1)
- aortic valve stenosis (1)
- applicant reactions (1)
- applicants for international protection (1)
- applied social psychology (1)
- arbeitsbezogene Belastungen (1)
- artifact correction (1)
- associative memory (1)
- asylum seekers (1)
- asynchronous video interviewing (1)
- attachment (1)
- attention shifting (1)
- attentional control (1)
- attitudes towards sexual coercion (1)
- attribution (1)
- attributions (1)
- audition (1)
- autocorrelations (1)
- automatic (1)
- automatic associations (1)
- automatic attitudes (1)
- automatic processes (1)
- automatic processing (1)
- automatische Prozesse (1)
- automotive design (1)
- aversion to clients (1)
- balance (1)
- basic reading capacities (1)
- basin evolution (1)
- bedingte Wahrscheinlichkeit (1)
- behavioral observation (1)
- behavioral reasoning (1)
- benefits (1)
- benzodiazepines (1)
- bilingual production and recognition (1)
- bilingualism (1)
- bilinguals (1)
- binding (1)
- binocular combination (1)
- bipolar (1)
- body weight (1)
- bottom-up (1)
- brain development (1)
- breast cancer survivors (1)
- capacity (1)
- career (1)
- career entry (1)
- career success (1)
- catastrophizing (1)
- categorization (1)
- category (1)
- central bottleneck (1)
- chaining (1)
- characteristics (1)
- child / adolescent / mental-health problem (1)
- child and adolescent psychotherapy (1)
- child's eating behavior (1)
- child's food intake (1)
- children at risk (1)
- chronic abdominal pain (1)
- chronic pain (1)
- chronic stress (1)
- chronisch entzündliche Darmerkrankungen (1)
- class-level effects (1)
- classification accuracy (1)
- climate (1)
- clinical interview (1)
- clinical psychology (1)
- clinical sample (1)
- clinical supervision (1)
- cognitive abilities (1)
- cognitive aging (1)
- cognitive behavioral intervention (1)
- cognitive bias (1)
- cognitive enhancement (1)
- cognitive flexibility (1)
- cognitive functions (1)
- cognitive speed (1)
- cognitive therapy (1)
- cognitive vulnerability (1)
- cognitive-behavioural therapy (1)
- cognitive-postural dual task (1)
- cohort differences (1)
- cold pressor pain (1)
- collective efficacy (1)
- common sense model of illness representation (1)
- comorbidities (1)
- comorbidity (1)
- competence development (1)
- competence need satisfaction (1)
- complex sentence processing (1)
- composition effects (1)
- compositional skills (1)
- compound detection or discrimination (1)
- computer using (1)
- computer-assisted self-regulation training (1)
- concepts of illness (1)
- conditionals (1)
- conduct disorder (1)
- confirmation bias (1)
- congruity effect (1)
- contingency approach (1)
- cool executive functioning (1)
- coreference (1)
- coregistration (1)
- corpus linguistics (1)
- corrective saccades (1)
- counting direction (1)
- couples (1)
- covert attention (1)
- critical race theory (1)
- cross-cultural (1)
- cross-cultural comparison (1)
- cross-cultural differences (1)
- cross-domain influences (1)
- cross-education (1)
- cross-lagged effects (1)
- cross-lagged panel analysis (1)
- crowding (1)
- cultural diversity climate (1)
- culture (1)
- deaf readers (1)
- decision making (1)
- decision-making (1)
- declarative memory (1)
- decoding process (1)
- deep learning (1)
- defendant liability (1)
- delayed parafoveal-on-foveal effects (1)
- dependence (1)
- dependency resolution (1)
- depressive Symptome (1)
- desensitization (1)
- developmental dyslexia (1)
- developmental task (1)
- diagnostic instruments for language assessment (1)
- diagnostische Kompetenz (1)
- diary study (1)
- diet (1)
- diffusion model (1)
- direct matching (1)
- disease management (1)
- disengagement (1)
- display change awareness (1)
- display size effect (1)
- display-change awareness (1)
- distinctiveness (1)
- dlex (1)
- dlexDB (1)
- domain specific knowledge (1)
- dominance effects (1)
- doping (1)
- drug instrumentalization (1)
- dual-process (1)
- dyadic coping (1)
- dynamic reciprocal relationship (1)
- dysfunctional attitudes (1)
- dysfunctional attitudies (1)
- early adolescence (1)
- early diagnosis (1)
- early numeracy (1)
- early smoking experiences (1)
- education (1)
- effects of trial history (1)
- efficacy (1)
- ego depletion (1)
- electrodermal activity (1)
- elementary school (1)
- elementary school-age students (1)
- elite athlete (1)
- elterliche Steuerungsstrategien (1)
- embodied numerical cognition (1)
- emergency (1)
- emotion control (1)
- emotion regulation (1)
- emotion-cognition interactions (1)
- emotional exhaustion (1)
- emotional intelligence (1)
- emotional valence (1)
- employee withdrawal (1)
- empowerment (1)
- encoding (1)
- engagement (1)
- enjoyment (1)
- entrainment (1)
- enumeration (1)
- episodic accumulators (1)
- episodic memory (1)
- episodische Akkumulatoren (1)
- ereigniskorrelierte Potentiale (1)
- ethnic identity (1)
- ethnicity (1)
- etiology (1)
- eudaimonia (1)
- eve movement (1)
- event-related potentials (ERP) (1)
- evoked potentials (1)
- evolutionary psychology (1)
- executive control processes (1)
- exekutive Funktionen (1)
- exekutive Kontrollprozesse (1)
- exercise tolerance (1)
- expectancy-value theory (1)
- experience sampling method (1)
- experiment (1)
- experimental evaluation (1)
- expertise (1)
- explicit (1)
- explizit (1)
- exploratory structural equation modeling (1)
- exposure therapy (1)
- external motivation (1)
- externale Kontrollüberzeugungen (1)
- externalizing problem behavior (1)
- eye movements and reading (1)
- eye movements during reading (1)
- eye-movement monitoring (1)
- eye-movements (1)
- eyetracking (1)
- face recognition (1)
- factor structure (1)
- familiarity (1)
- family background (1)
- fear of progression (1)
- fehlplazierte Fixationen (1)
- female perpetrators (1)
- femininity (1)
- fitness (1)
- fixation (1)
- fixation duration (1)
- fixation location (1)
- fixation positions (1)
- fixation sequences (1)
- fixation-related potentials (1)
- flow experience (1)
- flow-experience (1)
- flow-short-scale (1)
- fluid IQ and chronic stress exposure (1)
- fluid intelligence (1)
- food addiction (1)
- food approach (1)
- food preference (1)
- forced fixations (1)
- foreign language (1)
- formal cognitive models (1)
- formale kognitive Modelle (1)
- free association (1)
- free associations (1)
- free term entry (1)
- frequency (1)
- fructose malabsorption (1)
- fruits and vegetables (1)
- frühe Adoleszenz (1)
- functional abdominal pain (1)
- functional abdominal pain disorders (1)
- functions (1)
- gaze (1)
- gemischte Modelle (1)
- gender differences (1)
- gender stereotype (1)
- gender-sensitive didactics (1)
- gene x environment (1)
- general learning difficulty (1)
- general learning model (1)
- generalizability (1)
- genetics (1)
- geomorphic analysis (1)
- gesunde und ungesunde Nahrung (1)
- goal effectiveness training (1)
- goal orientation (1)
- goals (1)
- grade point average (1)
- grade retention (1)
- grounded cognition (1)
- growth-curve model (1)
- guilt (1)
- habitual and goal-directed system (1)
- hacker (1)
- hand dynamics (1)
- happiness/satisfaction (1)
- health (1)
- health at school (1)
- health behaviors (1)
- health of teachers (1)
- health resources (1)
- health-related cognition (1)
- healthy and unhealthy food (1)
- heart rate (1)
- heart rate variability (1)
- heartbeat perception (1)
- helping (1)
- heterogeneity (1)
- heuristics (1)
- higher education (1)
- honologische Bewusstheit (1)
- hot and cool executive function (1)
- human resource management (1)
- hydrogen breath test (1)
- illness anxiety disorder (1)
- illness representations (1)
- imitation (1)
- implicit (1)
- implicit measures (1)
- implicit motives (1)
- implicit theory of intelligence (1)
- implizit (1)
- implizite Motive (1)
- incentives (1)
- incision (1)
- incoming word predictability effect (1)
- independence goals (1)
- individual differences (personality, sex, age) (1)
- infancy (1)
- infants (1)
- inflammatory bowel disease (1)
- innate number sense (1)
- innovativeness (1)
- institutional discrimination (1)
- integration cost (1)
- intelligence (1)
- interactive learning environment (1)
- interalizing problem behavior (1)
- interference (1)
- internalizing problems (1)
- interoception (1)
- interoceptive awareness (1)
- interoceptive sensitivity (1)
- intervention program (1)
- interview method (1)
- irritability (1)
- job stress (1)
- junge Erwachsene (1)
- kindliche Ernährung (1)
- knowledge (1)
- knowledge elicitation (1)
- kognitive Funktionen (1)
- kognitive Vulnerabilität (1)
- kognitives Altern (1)
- lactose intolerance (1)
- language acquisition (1)
- language background (1)
- language diagnostics (1)
- language inhibition (1)
- language network (1)
- language switching (1)
- latent change score (1)
- latent transition analysis/latent profile analysis (1)
- leaking (1)
- learning and cognition (1)
- learning disability (1)
- learning progression (1)
- learning strategies (1)
- legal decision making (1)
- leisure motivation (1)
- leisure time (1)
- levels-of-inattention hypothesis (1)
- lexical database (1)
- lexical decision (1)
- lexical stress (1)
- life satisfaction (1)
- life span research (1)
- life stress (1)
- linear mixed models (1)
- lines of defense (1)
- long-term follow-up (1)
- longitudinal methodology (1)
- longitudinal research (1)
- male victims (1)
- management (1)
- masculinity (1)
- maternal distress (1)
- maternal feeding behavior (1)
- maternal perception (1)
- math disability (1)
- mathematical model (1)
- measurement (1)
- media choice (1)
- media competence (1)
- media use (1)
- mediation (1)
- memory access (1)
- memory clinic (1)
- mental chronometry (1)
- mental health-oriented work analysis (1)
- mental modell theory (1)
- mental number representation (1)
- mental number representations (1)
- mental stress (1)
- mental workload (1)
- mental-health (1)
- mentale Beanspruchung (1)
- mentale Modelle (1)
- mentaler Zahlenstrahl (1)
- meta-analysis (1)
- meter (1)
- method of places (1)
- microsaccade (1)
- microsaccades (1)
- middle aged / mental health (1)
- migrants (1)
- migration (1)
- mind wandering (1)
- mindless reading (1)
- mixed effects modeling (1)
- mnemonic skill (1)
- modality compatibility (1)
- modality pairings (1)
- model (1)
- model of Bayesian saccade planning (1)
- model-based and model-free learning (1)
- model-based learning (1)
- model-free learning (1)
- modelling (1)
- monocular deprivation (1)
- moral disengagement (1)
- mothers (1)
- mother–child interaction (1)
- motion discrimination (1)
- motivation in mathematics (1)
- motives (1)
- mousetracking (1)
- movement preparation (1)
- multi-level correlates (1)
- multicultural policy (1)
- multiethnic schools (1)
- multilevel modelling (1)
- multinomial models (1)
- muscle enhancement (1)
- muscularity concern (1)
- music (1)
- music cognition (1)
- music information retrieval (1)
- music perception (1)
- musical rhythm (1)
- n back (1)
- n+2-boundary paradigm (1)
- narrative writing (1)
- natural reading (1)
- natural viewing (1)
- natürliches Lesen (1)
- near-hand effect (1)
- need for action (1)
- negative numbers (1)
- neural oscillations (1)
- neuroenhancement (1)
- neurofeedback (1)
- neuronale Oszillationen (1)
- neuropeptide Y (1)
- new technology (1)
- nicotine (1)
- nomological network (1)
- non-addictive behavior (1)
- non-athletes (1)
- non-linear mixed effects (1)
- normative beliefs (1)
- number cognition (1)
- number line (1)
- numerische Basisfähigkeiten (1)
- nutritional supplements (1)
- observation (1)
- occlusion (1)
- occupational burden (1)
- occupational choices (1)
- occupational health (1)
- occupational therapists (1)
- oculomotor control (1)
- old adults and young adults (1)
- old age (1)
- old/new effect (1)
- on-line processing (1)
- optimal and preferred viewing position (1)
- optimism (1)
- optimization (1)
- organizational development (1)
- overspill (1)
- pain (1)
- pain threshold (1)
- pain tolerance (1)
- pair correlation function (1)
- panic (1)
- parabrachial nucleus (1)
- parafovea (1)
- parafoveal preview (1)
- parafoveal-on-foveal effects (1)
- parafoveale Verarbeitung (1)
- parafoveale Wortverarbeitung (1)
- parallel processing (1)
- parallel updating (1)
- parallele Aktualisierung (1)
- parent-child-association (1)
- parental feeding strategies (1)
- parental pressure (1)
- parenting (1)
- participation disorder (1)
- participation disorders (1)
- path analysis (1)
- patient education (1)
- peer influences (1)
- peer pressure (1)
- peer relationships (1)
- peers (1)
- perceived overprotection (1)
- perception (1)
- perception and action (1)
- perception-action-coupling (1)
- personal goals (1)
- personal initiative (1)
- personal relationships (1)
- personality disorder (1)
- persönliche Ziele (1)
- physical activity questionnaire (1)
- physical aggression (1)
- physical attractiveness (1)
- physical size (1)
- physicians (1)
- physiological arousal (1)
- physische Attraktivität (1)
- picture story exercise (1)
- pictures (1)
- placebo (1)
- pleasurable smoking sensations (1)
- point process (1)
- posterior statistical power (1)
- postpartum depression (1)
- postural stability (1)
- power (1)
- preadolescent depression (1)
- predictability (1)
- predicting stress (1)
- premotor (1)
- preschoolers (2-4 years) (1)
- primary care (1)
- proactive aggression (1)
- proactive behavior (1)
- proactive interference (1)
- proactive personality (1)
- proactive work behaviour (1)
- proaktive Interferenz (1)
- proaktives Verhalten (1)
- probabilistic (1)
- probabilistic theory (1)
- probabilistisch (1)
- probability of replication (1)
- probability summation (1)
- problematic eating behavior (1)
- problemorientiertes Lernen (1)
- process research (1)
- product experience (1)
- professional competency (1)
- pronoun resolution (1)
- propensity score matching (1)
- prosocial media (1)
- prosodic boundary (1)
- prosody (1)
- prostate cancer (1)
- proverbs (1)
- psychoactive drugs (1)
- psycholinguistics (1)
- psychological detachment (1)
- psychological distress (1)
- psychometric functions (1)
- psychophysics (1)
- psychophysiology (1)
- psychotherapy training (1)
- publication bias (1)
- questionnaire (1)
- random-effects model (1)
- randomized controlled trial (1)
- randomized-controlled trial (1)
- rape judgments (1)
- rape myths (1)
- rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) (1)
- reactive aggression (1)
- reading aloud (1)
- reading competence (1)
- reading comprehension (1)
- reading frequency (1)
- reading habits (1)
- reading speed (1)
- real-life events (1)
- reappraisal (1)
- reasoning (1)
- recognition memory (1)
- recreational sport (1)
- reduction (1)
- redundancy gain (1)
- referential coding (1)
- referential competence (1)
- referentielle Kompetenz (1)
- refugees (1)
- regression analysis (1)
- regression model (1)
- regressive Sakkaden (1)
- regressive saccades (1)
- rehabilitation episodes (1)
- reliability (1)
- replicability (1)
- representation learning (1)
- research transparency (1)
- reserve capacity (1)
- resistance training (1)
- resources (1)
- response error (1)
- response selection (1)
- retrieval (1)
- reverse correlation (1)
- review (1)
- reward (1)
- risk (1)
- risk and resilience (1)
- risk factors (1)
- role congruity theory (1)
- rs16147 (1)
- räumliche Orientierung (1)
- saccadic error (1)
- salience (1)
- saliency (1)
- same-sex contacts (1)
- scanpaths (1)
- school adjustment (1)
- school attack (1)
- school attacks (1)
- school belonging (1)
- school commitment (1)
- school leadership (1)
- school-related developmental disorder (1)
- schools (1)
- schulische Führung (1)
- schulische Gesundheit (1)
- schulischer Misserfolg (1)
- secondary education (1)
- secondary saccades (1)
- secondary school (1)
- selbst-attribuierte Motive (1)
- selbstregulative Komponenten (1)
- selection (1)
- selective exposure (1)
- self-attributed motives (1)
- self-concept (1)
- self-control (1)
- self-determination theory (1)
- self-developement (1)
- self-esteem (1)
- self-management (1)
- self-perceived ability (1)
- self-regulated learning (1)
- self-regulatory procedures (1)
- selfefficacy (1)
- semantic distance (1)
- semantic preview benefit (1)
- semantic priming (1)
- semantics (1)
- sensory balance (1)
- sequence learning (1)
- serial mediation (1)
- serial order memory (1)
- sexual victimisation (1)
- sexuelle Orientierung (1)
- shape concern (1)
- short-term food deprivation (1)
- short-term memory (1)
- short-term-intervention (1)
- sickness absence (1)
- signal prevalence (1)
- situational requirements (1)
- skill (1)
- small target (1)
- social behavior (1)
- social cognitive career theory (1)
- social interaction (1)
- social pressure (1)
- social responsibility (1)
- social situation (1)
- socialization effects (1)
- socioeconomic status (1)
- sonography (1)
- soziale Situation (1)
- soziale Unterstützung (1)
- soziale Zurückweisung (1)
- spatial correlations (1)
- spatial metaphors (1)
- spatial numerical associations (1)
- spatial orientation (1)
- spatial representation (1)
- spatial-nunmerical association (1)
- specific developmental disorder (1)
- speech processing (1)
- speech rhythm (1)
- sport (1)
- sprachdiagnostische Verfahren (1)
- statins (1)
- statistical estimation (1)
- statistical learning (1)
- stereotype (1)
- stereotype visualization (1)
- stimulus-onset asynchrony (1)
- stimulus-onset delay (1)
- stimulus-response mapping (1)
- storage cost (1)
- strategic termination (1)
- strength (1)
- strengths-based intervention (1)
- stressor (1)
- students (1)
- study-test congruence (1)
- stärkenbasierte Intervention (1)
- subjektive Krankheitskonzepte (1)
- substance use (1)
- suicidality (1)
- supervisor support (1)
- surgery (1)
- swift (1)
- symbolic calculation (1)
- sympathovagal balance (1)
- symposium (1)
- syntactic processing (1)
- syntaktische Ambiguität (1)
- syntax (1)
- tactile perception (1)
- task requirements (1)
- task switching (1)
- taste processing (1)
- teacher (1)
- teacher judgement accuracy (1)
- teacher judgment accuracy (1)
- teacher training (1)
- teacher training students (1)
- teachers (1)
- teachers health (1)
- teaching experience (1)
- team support (1)
- technology acceptance (1)
- technology acceptance model (1)
- technology commitment (1)
- technology competence (1)
- technology control (1)
- technology use (1)
- temperament (1)
- testing the limits (1)
- testing-the-limits (1)
- text analysis (1)
- text comprehension (1)
- thematic analysis (1)
- theory of goal systems (1)
- tie problems (1)
- time pressure (1)
- tool use (1)
- top-down (1)
- tracking (1)
- training (1)
- training adaptation (1)
- training evaluation (1)
- trait-anxiety (1)
- trajectories (1)
- transcranial magnetic stimulation (1)
- transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (1)
- transfer (1)
- transition economy (1)
- trial history (1)
- two visual systems (1)
- ulcerative colitis (1)
- unfinished tasks (1)
- unipolare affektive Störungen (1)
- updating (1)
- user types (1)
- validation of method (1)
- value education (1)
- value priorities (1)
- values (1)
- variability (1)
- verbal working memory (1)
- verbales Arbeitsgedächtnis (1)
- victim (1)
- victim blaming (1)
- video games (1)
- violence (1)
- violence in schools (1)
- violent media (1)
- violent pornography (1)
- visual span profiles (1)
- visual word recognition (1)
- visuelle Worterkennung (1)
- warning sign (1)
- week-level study (1)
- weight (1)
- weight concern (1)
- weight regulation (1)
- wh Questions (1)
- word associations (1)
- word boundaries (1)
- word learning (1)
- word recognition (1)
- word segmentation (1)
- word skipping (1)
- words (1)
- work characteristics (1)
- work experience (1)
- work values (1)
- work-related rumination (1)
- work-related stress (1)
- working-memory capacity (1)
- workplace phobia (1)
- worries and concerns (1)
- wrap-up process (1)
- writing strategies (1)
- x Comprehension (1)
- x Early adolescence (1)
- x Intrinsic (1)
- x Motivation/engagement, x Extrinsic (1)
- young people (1)
- youth (1)
- zBMI (1)
- zentraler Flaschenhals (1)
- Ähnlichkeit (1)
- Älterer Mensch / Psychische Gesundheit (1)
- Ängste und Sorgen (1)
- Ärzte (1)
- Ätiologie (1)
- Æ Recurrence Plots (1)
- Überspringen von Wörtern (1)
- ästhetische Nutzeranforderungen (1)
- ästhetische Präferenzen (1)
Institute
- Department Psychologie (1861) (remove)
Early numeracy is one of the strongest predictors for later success in school mathematics (e.g., Duncan et al., 2007). The main goal of first grade mathematics teachers should therefore be to provide learning opportunities that enable all students to develop sound early numeracy skills. Developmental models, or learning progressions, can describe how early numerical understanding typically develops. Assessments that are aligned to empirically validated learning progressions can support teachers to understand their students learning better and target instruction accordingly. To date, there have been no progression-based instruments made available for German teachers to monitor their students’ progress in the domain of early numeracy. This dissertation contributes to the design of such an instrument. The first study analysed the suitability of early numeracy assessments currently used in German primary schools at school entry to identify students’ individual starting points for subsequent progress monitoring. The second study described the development of progression-based items and investigated the items in regards to main test quality criteria, such as reliability, validity, and test fairness, to find a suitable item pool to build targeted tests. The third study described the construction of the progress monitoring measure, referred to as the learning progress assessment (LPA). The study investigated the extent to which the LPA was able to monitor students’ individual learning progress in early numeracy over time. The results of the first study indicated that current school entry assessments were not able to provide meaningful information about the students’ initial learning status. Thus, the MARKO-D test (Ricken, Fritz, & Balzer, 2013) was used to determine the students’ initial numerical understanding in the other two studies, because it has been shown to be an effective measure of conceptual numerical understanding (Fritz, Ehlert, & Leutner, 2018). Both studies provided promising evidence for the quality of the LPA and its ability to detect changes in numerical understanding over the course of first grade. The studies of this dissertation can be considered an important step in the process of designing an empirically validated instrument that supports teachers to monitor their students’ early numeracy development and to adjust their teaching accordingly to enhance school achievement.
The decision to exercise is not only bound to rational considerations but also automatic affective processes. The affective–reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise (ART) proposes a theoretical framework for explaining how the automatic affective process (type‑1 process) will influence exercise behavior, i.e., through the automatic activation of exercise-related associations and a subsequent affective valuation of exercise. This study aimed to empirically test this assumption of the ART with data from 69 study participants. A single-measurement study, including within-subject experimental variation, was conducted. Automatic associations with exercise were first measured with a single-target implicit association test. The somato-affective core of the participants’ automatic valuation of exercise-related pictures was then assessed via heart rate variability (HRV) analysis, and the affective valence of the valuation was tested with a facial expression (FE; smile and frown) task. Exercise behavior was assessed via self-report. Multiple regression (path) analysis revealed that automatic associations predicted HRV reactivity (β = −0.24, p = .044); the signs of the correlation between automatic associations and the smile FE score was in the expected direction but remained nonsignificant (β = −0.21, p = .078). HRV reactivity predicted self-reported exercise behavior (β = −0.28, p = .013) (the same pattern of results was achieved for the frown FE score). The HRV-related results illustrate the potential role of automatic negative affective reactions to the thought of exercise as a restraining force in exercise motivation. For better empirical distinction between the two ART type‑1 process components, automatic associations and the affective valuation should perhaps be measured separately in the future. The results support the notion that automatic and affective processes should be regarded as essential aspects of the motivation to exercise.
The decision to exercise is not only bound to rational considerations but also automatic affective processes. The affective–reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise (ART) proposes a theoretical framework for explaining how the automatic affective process (type‑1 process) will influence exercise behavior, i.e., through the automatic activation of exercise-related associations and a subsequent affective valuation of exercise. This study aimed to empirically test this assumption of the ART with data from 69 study participants. A single-measurement study, including within-subject experimental variation, was conducted. Automatic associations with exercise were first measured with a single-target implicit association test. The somato-affective core of the participants’ automatic valuation of exercise-related pictures was then assessed via heart rate variability (HRV) analysis, and the affective valence of the valuation was tested with a facial expression (FE; smile and frown) task. Exercise behavior was assessed via self-report. Multiple regression (path) analysis revealed that automatic associations predicted HRV reactivity (β = −0.24, p = .044); the signs of the correlation between automatic associations and the smile FE score was in the expected direction but remained nonsignificant (β = −0.21, p = .078). HRV reactivity predicted self-reported exercise behavior (β = −0.28, p = .013) (the same pattern of results was achieved for the frown FE score). The HRV-related results illustrate the potential role of automatic negative affective reactions to the thought of exercise as a restraining force in exercise motivation. For better empirical distinction between the two ART type‑1 process components, automatic associations and the affective valuation should perhaps be measured separately in the future. The results support the notion that automatic and affective processes should be regarded as essential aspects of the motivation to exercise.
The PhD thesis entitled “Actions through the lens of communicative cues. The influence of verbal cues and emotional cues on action processing and action selection in the second year of life” is based on four studies, which examined the cognitive integration of another person’s communicative cues (i.e., verbal cues, emotional cues) with behavioral cues in 18- and 24-month-olds. In the context of social learning of instrumental actions, it was investigated how the intention-related coherence of either a verbally announced action intention or an emotionally signaled action evaluation with an action demonstration influenced infants’ neuro-cognitive processing (Study I) and selection (Studies II, III, IV) of a novel object-directed action. Developmental research has shown that infants benefit from another’s behavioral cues (e.g., action effect, persistency, selectivity) to infer the underlying goal or intention, respectively, of an observed action (e.g., Cannon & Woodward, 2012; Woodward, 1998). Particularly action effects support infants in distinguishing perceptual action features (e.g., target object identity, movement trajectory, final target object state) from conceptual action features such as goals and intentions. However, less is known about infants’ ability to cognitively integrate another’s behavioral cues with additional action-related communicative cues. There is some evidence showing that in the second year of life, infants selectively imitate a novel action that is verbally (“There!”) or emotionally (positive expression) marked as aligning with the model’s action intention over an action that is verbally (“Whoops!”) or emotionally (negative expression) marked as unintentional (Carpenter, Akhtar, & Tomasello, 1998; Olineck & Poulin-Dubois, 2005, 2009; Repacholi, 2009; Repacholi, Meltzoff, Toub, & Ruba, 2016). Yet, it is currently unclear which role the specific intention-related coherence of a communicative cue with a behavioral cue plays in infants’ action processing and action selection that is, whether the communicative cue confirms, contrasts, clarifies, or is unrelated to the behavioral cue. Notably, by using both verbal cues and emotional cues, we examined not only two domains of communicative cues but also two qualitatively distinct relations between behavioral cues on the one hand and communicative cues on the other hand. More specifically, a verbal cue has the potential to communicate an action intention in the absence of an action demonstration and thus a prior-intention (Searle, 1983), whereas an emotional cue evaluates an ongoing or past action demonstration and thus signals an intention-in-action (Searle, 1983). In a first research focus, this thesis examined infants’ capacity to cognitively integrate another’s intention-related communicative cues and behavioral cues, and also focused on the role of the social cues’ coherence in infants’ action processing and action selection. In a second research focus, and to gain more elaborate insights into how the sub-processes of social learning (attention, encoding, response; cf. Bandura, 1977) are involved in this coherence-sensitive integrative processing, we employed a multi-measures approach. More specifically, we used Electroencephalography (EEG) and looking times to examine how the cues’ coherence influenced the compound of attention and encoding, and imitation (including latencies to first-touch and first-action) to address the compound of encoding and response. Based on the action-reconstruction account (Csibra, 2007), we predicted that infants use extra-motor information (i.e., communicative cues) together with behavioral cues to reconstruct another’s action intention. Accordingly, we expected infants to possess a flexibly organized internal action hierarchy, which they adapt according to the cues’ coherence that is, according to what they inferred to be the overarching action goal. More specifically, in a social-learning situation that comprised an adult model, who demonstrated an action on a novel object that offered two actions, we expected the demonstrated action to lead infants’ action hierarchy when the communicative (i.e., verbal, emotional) cue conveyed similar (confirming coherence) or no additional (un-related coherence) intention-related information relative to the behavioral cue. In terms of action selection, this action hierarchy should become evident in a selective imitation of the demonstrated action. However, when the communicative cue questioned (contrasting coherence) the behaviorally implied action goal or was the only cue conveying meaningful intention-related information (clarifying coherence), the verbally/emotionally intended action should ascend infants’ action hierarchy. Consequently, infants’ action selection should align with the verbally/emotionally intended action (goal emulation). Notably, these predictions oppose the direct-matching perspective (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), according to which the observation of another’s action directly resonates with the observer’s motor repertoire, with this motor resonance enabling the identification of the underlying action goal. Importantly, the direct-matching perspective predicts a rather inflexible action hierarchy inasmuch as the process of goal identification should solely rely on the behavioral cue, irrespective of the behavioral cue’s coherence with extra-motor intention-related information, as it may be conveyed via communicative cues. As to the role of verbal cues, Study I used EEG to examine the influence of a confirming (Congruent) versus contrasting (Incongruent) coherence of a verbal action intention with the same action demonstration on 18-month-olds’ conceptual action processing (as measured via mid-latency mean negative ERP amplitude) and motor activation (as measured via central mu-frequency band power). The action was demonstrated on a novel object that offered two action alternatives from a neutral position. We expected mid-latency ERP negativity to be enhanced in Incongruent compared to Congruent, because past EEG research has demonstrated enhanced conceptual processing for stimuli that mismatched rather than matched the semantic context (Friedrich & Friederici, 2010; Kaduk et al., 2016). Regarding motor activation, Csibra (2007) posited that the identification of a clear action goal constitutes a crucial basis for motor activation to occur. We therefore predicted reduced mu power (indicating enhanced motor activation) for Congruent than Incongruent, because in Congruent, the cues’ match provides unequivocal information about the model’s action goal, whereas in Incongruent, the conflict may render the model’s action goal more unclear. Unexpectedly, in the entire sample, 18-month-olds’ mid-latency ERP negativity during the observation of the same action demonstration did not differ significantly depending on whether this action was congruent or incongruent with the model’s verbal action intention. Yet, post hoc analyses revealed the presence of two subgroups of infants, each of which exhibited significantly different mid-latency ERP negativity for Congruent versus Incongruent, but in opposing directions. The subgroups differed in their productive action-related language skills, with the linguistically more advanced infants exhibiting the expected response pattern of enhanced ERP mean negativity in Incongruent than Congruent, indicating enhanced conceptual processing of an action demonstration that was contrasted rather than confirmed by the verbal action context. As expected, central mu power in the entire sample was reduced in Congruent relative to Incongruent, indicating enhanced motor activation when the action demonstration was preceded by a confirming relative to a contrasting verbal action intention. This finding may indicate the covert preparation for a preferential imitation of the congruent relative to the incongruent action (Filippi et al., 2016; Frey & Gerry, 2006). Overall, these findings are in line with the action-reconstruction account (Csibra, 2007), because they suggest a coherence-sensitive attention to and encoding of the same perceptual features of another’s behavior and thus a cognitive integration of intention-related verbal cues and behavioral cues. Yet, because the subgroup constellation in infants’ ERPs was only discovered post hoc, future research is clearly required to substantiate this finding. Also, future research should validate our interpretation that enhanced motor activation may reflect an electrophysiological marker of subsequent imitation by employing EEG and imitation in a within-subjects design. Study II built on Study I by investigating the impact of coherence of a verbal cue and a behavioral cue on 18- and 24-month-olds’ action selection in an imitation study. When infants of both age groups observed a confirming (Congruent) or unrelated (Pseudo-word: action demonstration was associated with novel verb-like cue) coherence, they selectively imitated the demonstrated action over the not demonstrated, alternative action, with no difference between these two conditions. These findings suggest that, as expected, infants’ action hierarchy was led by the demonstrated action when the verbal cue provided similar (Congruent) or no additional (Pseudo-word) intention-related information relative to a meaningful behavioral cue. These findings support the above-mentioned interpretation that enhanced motor activation during action observation may reflect a covert preparation for imitation (Study I). Interestingly, infants did not seem to benefit from the intention-highlighting effect of the verbal cue in Congruent, suggesting that the verbal cue had an unspecific (e.g., attention-guiding) effect on infants’ action selection. Contrary, when infants observed a contrasting (Incongruent) or clarifying (Failed-attempt: model failed to manipulate the object but verbally announced a certain action intention) coherence, their action selection varied with age and also varied across the course of the experiment (block 1 vs. block 2). More specifically, the 24-month-olds made stronger use of the verbal cue for their action selection in block 1 than did the 18-month-olds. However, while the 18-month-olds’ use of the verbal cue increased across blocks, particularly in Incongruent, the 24-month-olds’ use of the verbal cue decreased across blocks. Overall, these results suggest that, as expected, infants’ action hierarchy in Incongruent (both age groups) and Failed-attempt (only 24-month-olds) drew on the verbal action intention, because in both age groups, infants emulated the verbal intention about as often as they imitated the demonstrated action or even emulated the verbal action intention preferentially. Yet, these findings were confined to certain blocks. It may be argued that the younger age group had a harder time inferring and emulating the intended, yet never observed action, because this requirement is more demanding in cognitive and motor terms. These demands may explain why the 18-month-olds needed some time to take account of the verbal action intention. Contrary, it seems that the 24-month-olds, although demonstrating their principle capacity to take account of the verbal cue in block 1, lost trust in the model’s verbal cue, maybe because the verbal cue did not have predictive value for the model’s actual behavior. Supporting this interpretation, research on selective trust has demonstrated that already infants evaluate another’s reliability or competence, respectively, based on how that model handles familiar objects (behavioral reliability) or labels familiar objects (verbal reliability; for reviews, see Mills, 2013; Poulin-Dubois & Brosseau-Liard, 2016). Relatedly, imitation research has demonstrated that the interpersonal aspects of a social-learning situation gain increasing relevance for infants during the second year of life (Gellén & Buttelmann, 2019; Matheson, Moore, & Akhtar, 2013; Uzgiris, 1981). It may thus be argued that when the 24-month-olds were repeatedly faced with a verbally unreliable model, they de-evaluated the verbal cue as signaling the model’s action intention and instead relied more heavily on alternative cues such as the behavioral cue (Incongruent) or the action context (e.g., object affordances, salience; Failed-attempt). Infants’ first-action latencies were higher in Incongruent and Failed-attempt than in both Congruent and Pseudo-word, and were also higher in Failed-attempt than in Incongruent. These latency-findings thus indicate that situations involving a meaningful verbal cue that deviated from the behavioral cue are cognitively more demanding, resulting in a delayed initiation of a behavioral response. In sum, the findings of Study II suggest that both age groups were highly flexible in their integration of a verbal cue and behavioral cue. Moreover, our results do not indicate a general superiority of either cue. Instead, it seems to depend on the informational gain conveyed by the verbal cue whether it exerts a specific, intention-highlighting effect (Incongruent, Failed-attempt) or an unspecific (e.g., attention-guiding) effect (Congruent, Pseudo-word). Studies III and IV investigated the impact of another’s action-related emotional cues on 18-month-olds’ action selection. In Study III, infants observed a model, who demonstrated two actions on a novel object in direct succession, and who combined one of the two actions with a positive (happy) emotional expression and the other action with a negative (sad) emotional expression. As expected, infants imitated the positively emoted (PE) action more often than the negatively emoted (NE) action. This preference arose from an increase in infants’ readiness to perform the PE action from the baseline period (prior to the action demonstrations) to the test period (following the action demonstrations), rather than from a decrease in readiness to the perform the NE action. The positive cue thus had a stronger behavior-regulating effect than the negative cue. Notably, infants’ more general object-directed behavior in terms of first-touch latencies remained unaffected by the emotional cues’ valence, indicating that infants had linked the emotional cues specifically to the corresponding action and not the object as a whole (Repacholi, 2009). Also, infants’ looking times during the action demonstration did not differ significantly as a function of emotional valence and were characterized by a predominant attentional focus to the action/object rather than to the model’s face. Together with the findings on infants’ first-touch latencies, these results indicate a sensitivity for the notion that emotions can have very specific referents (referential specificity; Martin, Maza, McGrath, & Phelps, 2014). Together, Study III provided evidence for selective imitation based on another’s intention-related (particularly positive) emotional cues in an action-selection task, and thus indicates that infants’ action hierarchy flexibly responds to another’s emotional evaluation of observed actions. According to Repacholi (2009), we suggest that infants used the model’s emotional evaluation to re-appraise the corresponding action (effect), for instance in terms of desirability. Study IV followed up on Study III by investigating the role of the negative emotional cue for infants’ action selection in more detail. Specifically, we investigated whether a contrasting (negative) emotional cue alone would be sufficient to differentially rank the two actions along infants’ action hierarchy or whether instead infants require direct information about the model’s action intention (in the form of a confirming action-emotion pair) to align their action selection with the emotional cues. Also, we examined whether the absence of a direct behavior-regulating effect of the negative cue in Study III was due to the negative cue itself or to the concurrently available positive cue masking the negative cue’s potential effect. To this end, we split the demonstration of the two action-emotion pairs across two trials. In each trial, one action was thus demonstrated and emoted (PE, NE action), and one action was not demonstrated and un-emoted (UE action). For trial 1, we predicted that infants, who observed a PE action demonstration, would selectively imitate the PE action, whereas infants, who observed a NE action demonstration would selectively emulate the UE action. As to trial 2, we expected the complementary action-emotion pair to provide additional clarifying information as the model’s emotional evaluation of both actions, which should either lead to adaptive perseveration (if infants’ action selection in trial 1 had already drawn on the emotional cue) or adaptive change (if infants’ action selection in trial 1 signaled a disregard of the emotional cue). As to trial 1, our findings revealed that, as expected, infants imitated the PE action more often than they emulated the UE action. Like in Study III, this selectivity arose from an increase in infants’ propensity to perform the PE action from baseline to trial 1. Also like in Study III, infants performed the NE action about equally often in baseline and trial 1, which speaks against a direct behavior-regulating effect of the negative cue also when presented in isolation. However, after a NE action demonstration, infants emulated the UE action more often in trial 1 than in baseline, suggesting an indirect behavior-regulating effect of the negative cue. Yet, this indirect effect did not yield a selective emulation of the UE action, because infants performed both action alternatives about equally often in trial 1. Unexpectedly, infants’ action selection in trial 2 was unaffected by the emotional cue. Instead, infants perseverated their action selection of trial 1 in trial 2, irrespective of whether it was adaptive or non-adaptive with respect to the model’s emotional evaluation of the action. It seems that infants changed their strategy across trials, from an initial adherence to the emotional (particularly positive) cue, towards bringing about a salient action effect (Marcovich & Zelazo, 2009). In sum, Studies III and IV indicate a dynamic interplay of different action-selection strategies, depending on valence and presentation order. Apparently, at least in infancy, action reconstruction as one basis for selective action performance reaches its limits when infants can only draw on indirect intention-related information (i.e., which action should be avoided). Overall, our findings favor the action-reconstruction account (Csibra, 2007), according to which actions are flexibly organized along a hierarchy, depending on inferential processes based on extra-motor intention-related information. At the same time, the findings question the direct-matching hypothesis (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), according to which the identification (and pursuit) of action goals hinges on a direct simulation of another’s behavioral cues. Based on the studies’ findings, a preliminary working model is introduced, which seeks to integrate the two theoretical accounts by conceptualizing the routes that activation induced by social cues may take to eventually influence an infant’s action selection. Our findings indicate that it is useful to strive a differentiated conceptualization of communicative cues, because they seem to operate at different places within the process of cue integration, depending on their potential to convey direct intention-related information. Moreover, we suggest that there is bidirectional exchange within each compound of adjacent sub-processes (i.e., between attention and encoding, and encoding and response), and between the compounds. Hence, our findings highlight the benefits of a multi-measures approach when studying the development of infants’ social-cognitive abilities, because it provides a more comprehensive picture how the concerted use of social cues from different domains influences infants’ processing and selection of instrumental actions. Finally, this thesis points to potential future directions to substantiate our current interpretation of the findings.. Moreover, an extension to additional kinds of coherence is suggested to get closer to infants’ everyday-world of experience.
Even though the majority of individuals know that exercising is healthy, a high percentage struggle to achieve the recommended amount of exercise. The (social-cognitive) theories that are commonly applied to explain exercise motivation refer to the assumption that people base their decisions mainly on rational reasoning. However, behavior is not only bound to reflection. In recent years, the role of automaticity and affect for exercise motivation has been increasingly discussed. In this dissertation, central assumptions of the affective–reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise (ART; Brand & Ekkekakis, 2018), an exercise-specific dual-process theory that emphasizes the role of a momentary automatic affective reaction for exercise-decisions, were examined. The central aim of this dissertation was to investigate exercisers and non-exercisers automatic affective reactions to exercise-related stimuli (i.e., type-1 process). In particular, the two components of the ART’s type-1 process, that are, automatic associations with exercise and the automatic affective valuation to exercise, were under study.
In the first publication (Schinkoeth & Antoniewicz, 2017), research on automatic (evaluative) associations with exercise was summarized and evaluated in a systematic review. The results indicated that automatic associations with exercise appeared to be relevant predictors for exercise behavior and other exercise-related variables, providing evidence for a central assumption of the ART’s type-1 process. Furthermore, indirect methods seem to be suitable to assess automatic associations. The aim of the second publication (Schinkoeth, Weymar, & Brand, 2019) was to approach the somato-affective core of the automatic valuation of exercise using analysis of reactivity in vagal HRV while viewing exercise-related pictures. Results revealed that differences in exercise volume could be regressed on HRV reactivity. In light of the ART, these findings were interpreted as evidence of an inter-individual affective reaction elicited at the thought of exercise and triggered by exercise-stimuli. In the third publication (Schinkoeth & Brand, 2019, subm.), it was sought to disentangle and relate to each other the ART’s type-1 process components—automatic associations and the affective valuation of exercise. Automatic associations to exercise were assessed with a recoding-free variant of an implicit association test (IAT). Analysis of HRV reactivity was applied to approach a somatic component of the affective valuation, and facial reactions in a facial expression (FE) task served as indicators of the automatic affective reaction’s valence. Exercise behavior was assessed via self-report. The measurement of the affective valuation’s valence with the FE task did not work well in this study. HRV reactivity was predicted by the IAT score and did also statistically predict exercise behavior. These results thus confirm and expand upon the results of publication two and provide empirical evidence for the type-1 process, as defined in the ART. This dissertation advances the field of exercise psychology concerning the influence of automaticity and affect on exercise motivation. Moreover, both methodical implications and theoretical extensions for the ART can be derived from the results.
Minor cosmopolitan
(2020)
Cosmopolitanism is a theory about how to live together. The earliest formulation of cosmopolitanism in the West could be dated to as early as the fourth century BCE in ancient Greece by Diogenes, who famously said that he was a “citizen of the world – kosmopolitês,” an idea later picked up by Immanuel Kant, the German philosopher who proposed a philosophy of a world of “perpetual peace.” When cosmopolitanism first emerged as a political idea for modernity in the European Enlightenment, the project embraced the liberal promises of a globalizing economy, yet remained oblivious to, and even complicit with, capitalism, slavery and colonialism. It centered on the male, bourgeois, and white liberal subject, irrespective of the ongoing disenfranchisement, dehumanization, and extermination of its Others.
At the dawn of the 21st century, and in the wake of rapid globalization however, academics, politicians and other pundits enthusiastically declared cosmopolitanism to be no longer just a philosophical ideal, but a real, existing fact. Across the globe, they argued, people were increasingly thinking and feeling beyond the nation, considering themselves citizens of the world. Meanwhile, the global ecological crisis worsens, fascism with different outfits returns in many places of the world, the repression of women, sexual, racial, class and other minorities on a global scale persists; the so called “refugee crisis” inundates the mediascape and political spectacle. Not much of those cosmopolitan promises have left it seems. Perhaps precisely because of this, however, it seems to be an absolute necessity for scholars, activists, and artists today to face the complexities and promises cosmopolitanism has raised although not adequately answered. What has happened to the cosmopolitan promise, and who betrayed it?
“Minor cosmopolitanisms” wishes to challenge the underlying premises of ‘major’ cosmopolitanism without letting go of the unfulfilled emancipatory potential of the concept at large. It wants to rethink cosmopolitanisms in the plural, and trace multiple origins and trajectories of cosmopolitan thought from across the globe. Regarding cosmopolitanisms as emerging through diverse locally, historically and politically specific practices, minor cosmopolitanisms are predicated on difference without abandoning the quest for a shared vision of conviviality and justice. It seeks to answer: how to live at once with our difference and shared struggle? How to think our complicity with even those we most resist? Who sustains the world’s flourishing despite all this?
Thoroughly revised and updated, this third edition offers a comprehensive and up-to-date overview of the social psychology of aggression, covering all the relevant major theories, individual differences, situational factors, and applied contexts. Understanding the causes, forms, and consequences of aggression and violence is critical for dealing with these harmful forms of social behavior. Addressing a range of sub-topics, the firstpart deals with the definition and measurement of aggression, presents major theories, examines the development of aggression and discusses individual and gender differences in aggressive behaviour. It covers the role of situational factors in eliciting aggression and the impact of exposure to violence in the media. The second part examines specific forms and manifestations of aggression, including chapters on aggression in everyday contexts and in the family, sexual aggression, intergroup aggression, and terrorism. The new edition also includes additional coverage of gender differences, gun violence, and terrorism, to reflect the latest research developments in the field. Alsodiscussing strategies for reducing and preventing aggression, this bookis essential reading for students and researchers in psychology and related disciplines, as well as practitioners andpolicy makers.
Feministisches Spekulieren
(2020)
The interplay between cognitive and oculomotor processes during reading can be explored when the spatial layout of text deviates from the typical display. In this study, we investigate various eye-movement measures during reading of text with experimentally manipulated layout (word-wise and letter-wise mirrored-reversed text as well as inverted and scrambled text). While typical findings (e.g., longer mean fixation times, shorter mean saccades lengths) in reading manipulated texts compared to normal texts were reported in earlier work, little is known about changes of oculomotor targeting observed in within-word landing positions under the above text layouts. Here we carry out precise analyses of landing positions and find substantial changes in the so-called launch-site effect in addition to the expected overall slow-down of reading performance. Specifically, during reading of our manipulated text conditions with reversed letter order (against overall reading direction), we find a reduced launch-site effect, while in all other manipulated text conditions, we observe an increased launch-site effect. Our results clearly indicate that the oculomotor system is highly adaptive when confronted with unusual reading conditions.
Most reading theories assume that readers aim at word centers for optimal information processing. During reading, saccade targeting turns out to be imprecise: Saccades’ initial landing positions often miss the word centers and have high variance, with an additional systematic error that is modulated by the distance from the launch site to the center of the target word. The performance of the oculomotor system, as reflected in the statistics of within-word landing positions, turns out to be very robust and mostly affected by the spatial information during reading. Hence, it is assumed that the saccade generation is highly automated.
The main goal of this thesis is to explore the performance of the oculomotor system under various reading conditions where orthographic information and the reading direction were manipulated. Additionally, the challenges in understanding the eye movement data to represent the oculomotor process during reading are addressed.
Two experimental studies and one simulation study were conducted for this thesis, which resulted in the following main findings:
(i) Reading texts with orthographic manipulations leads to specific changes in the eye movement patterns, both in temporal and spatial measures. The findings indicate that the oculomotor control of eye movements during reading is dependent on reading conditions (Chapter 2 & 3).
(ii) Saccades’ accuracy and precision can be simultaneously modulated under reversed reading condition, supporting the assumption that the random and systematic oculomotor errors are not independent. By assuming that readers increase the precision of sensory observation while maintaining the learned prior knowledge when reading direction was reversed, a process-oriented Bayesian model for saccade targeting can account for the simultaneous reduction of oculomotor errors (Chapter 2).
(iii) Plausible parameter values serving as proxies for the intended within-word landing positions can be estimated by using the maximum a posteriori estimator from Bayesian inference. Using the mean value of all observations as proxies is insufficient for studies focusing on the launch-site effect because the method exhibits the strongest bias when estimating the size of the effect. Mislocated fixations remain a challenge for the currently known estimation methods, especially when the systematic oculomotor error is large (Chapter 4).
The results reported in this thesis highlight the role of the oculomotor system, together with underlying cognitive processes, in eye movements during reading. The modulation of oculomotor control can be captured through a precise analysis of landing positions.
During reading, rapid eye movements (saccades) shift the reader's line of sight from one word to another for high-acuity visual information processing. While experimental data and theoretical models show that readers aim at word centers, the eye-movement (oculomotor) accuracy is low compared to other tasks. As a consequence, distributions of saccadic landing positions indicate large (i) random errors and (ii) systematic over- and undershoot of word centers, which additionally depend on saccade lengths (McConkie et al.Visual Research, 28(10), 1107-1118,1988). Here we show that both error components can be simultaneously reduced by reading texts from right to left in German language (N= 32). We used our experimental data to test a Bayesian model of saccade planning. First, experimental data are consistent with the model. Second, the model makes specific predictions of the effects of the precision of prior and (sensory) likelihood. Our results suggest that it is a more precise sensory likelihood that can explain the reduction of both random and systematic error components.
The interplay between cognitive and oculomotor processes during reading can be explored when the spatial layout of text deviates from the typical display. In this study, we investigate various eye-movement measures during reading of text with experimentally manipulated layout (word-wise and letter-wise mirrored-reversed text as well as inverted and scrambled text). While typical findings (e.g., longer mean fixation times, shorter mean saccades lengths) in reading manipulated texts compared to normal texts were reported in earlier work, little is known about changes of oculomotor targeting observed in within-word landing positions under the above text layouts. Here we carry out precise analyses of landing positions and find substantial changes in the so-called launch-site effect in addition to the expected overall slow-down of reading performance. Specifically, during reading of our manipulated text conditions with reversed letter order (against overall reading direction), we find a reduced launch-site effect, while in all other manipulated text conditions, we observe an increased launch-site effect. Our results clearly indicate that the oculomotor system is highly adaptive when confronted with unusual reading conditions.
On being and belonging
(2019)
Listening to the heart
(2019)
Objective: The affective-reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise suggests that the mere thought of exercise can lead to an immediate somato-affective response which, if negative, will drive a physically inactive person to maintain his or her current exercise-avoidant behavior. This study aimed to test the assumption that the somatic core of this affective response can be identified by means of heart rate variability (HRV) analysis. Design: This study followed a within-subject experimental design. Method. Participants were 91 adult men and women whose HR and HRV were monitored whilst they viewed exercise-related and control pictures in a laboratory setting. Results: Analyses revealed a decrease in HRV during the viewing of exercise-related pictures in less physically active participants. These participants reported that the same pictures elicited feelings with relatively low affective valence and arousal. There were no changes in HR.
In this report, we illustrate the considerable impact of researcher degrees of freedom with respect to exclusion of participants in paradigms with a learning element. We illustrate this empirically through case examples from human fear conditioning research, in which the exclusion of ‘non-learners’ and ‘non-responders’ is common – despite a lack of consensus on how to define these groups. We illustrate the substantial heterogeneity in exclusion criteria identified in a systematic literature search and highlight the potential problems and pitfalls of different definitions through case examples based on re-analyses of existing data sets. On the basis of these studies, we propose a consensus on evidence-based rather than idiosyncratic criteria, including clear guidelines on reporting details. Taken together, we illustrate how flexibility in data collection and analysis can be avoided, which will benefit the robustness and replicability of research findings and can be expected to be applicable to other fields of research that involve a learning element.
Intolerance to lactose or fructose is frequently diagnosed in children with chronic abdominal pain (CAP). However, the causal relationship remains a matter of discussion. A cohort of 253 patients, aged 7-12 years, presenting with unexplained CAP received standardized diagnostics. Additional diagnostic tests were performed based on their medical history and physical and laboratory investigations. Fructose and lactose hydrogen breath tests (H2BT) as well as empiric diagnostic elimination diets were performed in 135 patients reporting abdominal pain related to the consumption of lactose or fructose to evaluate carbohydrate intolerance as a potential cause of CAP. Carbohydrate malabsorption by H2BT was found in 55 (41%) out of 135 patients. An abnormal increase in H2BT was revealed in 30% (35/118) of patients after fructose consumption and in 18% (20/114) of patients after lactose administration. Forty-six percent (25/54) reported pain relief during a diagnostic elimination diet. In total, 17 patients had lactose malabsorption, 29 fructose malabsorption, and nine combined carbohydrate malabsorption. Carbohydrate intolerance as a cause of CAP was diagnosed at follow-up in only 18% (10/55) of patients with malabsorption after the elimination of the respective carbohydrate. Thus, carbohydrate malabsorption appears to be an incidental finding in children with functional abdominal pain disorders, rather than its cause. Therefore, testing of carbohydrate intolerance should only be considered in children with a strong clinical suspicion and with the goal to prevent long-term unnecessary dietary restrictions in children suffering from CAP.
The present study sets out to address two fundamental questions in the reading of continuous texts: Whether semantic and phonological information from upcoming words can be accessed during natural reading. In the present study we investigated parafoveal processing during the reading of Korean sentences, manipulating semantic and phonological information from parafoveal preview words. In addition to the first evidence for a semantic preview effect in Korean, we found that Korean readers have stronger and more long-lasting phonological than semantic activation from parafoveal words in second-pass reading. The present study provides an example that human mind can flexibly adjust processing priority to different types of information based on the linguistic environment.
Previous research on dispositional optimism has predominantly concentrated on the selection effect of dispositional optimism on predicting work outcomes. Recent research, however, has started to examine the socialization effect of life experiences on fostering dispositional optimism development. Extrapolating primarily from the TESSERA framework of personality development (Wrzus & Roberts, 2017) and the literature on dispositional optimism, the current study represents a first attempt to reconcile the 2 seemingly contrasting perspectives. We proposed and examined change-related reciprocal relationships between dispositional optimism and work experience variables including income, job insecurity, coworker support. and supervisor support. Latent change score modeling of data from a five-wave longitudinal study demonstrated that dispositional optimism resulted in decreases in job insecurity, and the decreased job insecurity in turn promoted further increases in dispositional optimism later on. Furthermore, income gave rise to increases ill dispositional optimism at a later point in time. but not vice versa. No significant relationships were observed between dispositional optimism and coworker and supervisor support. The findings provide a cautionary note to the majority of previous research based on cross-sectional and lagged designs that assumes causal effects of dispositional optimism on work outcomes. They also showcase the importance of examining personality change in organizational research and enrich our understanding of a more nuanced dynamic interplay between the optimistic employee and the work environment.
The present study examines how historical changes in the U.S. socioeconomic environment in the 20th century may have affected core assumptions of the "American Dream." Specifically, the authors examined whether such changes modulated the extent to which adolescents' intelligence (IQ), their grade point average (GPA), and their parents' socioeconomic status (SES) could predict key life outcomes in adulthood about 20 years later. The data stemmed from two representative U.S. birth cohorts of 15- and 16-year-olds who were born in the early 1960s (N = 3,040) and 1980s (N = 3,524) and who participated in the National Longitudinal Surveys of Youth (NLSY). Cohort differences were analyzed with respect to differences in average relations by means of multiple and logistic regression and for specific points in each outcome distribution by means of quantile regressions. In both cohorts, IQ, GPA, and parental SES predicted important educational, occupational, and health-related life-outcomes about 20 years later. Across historical time, the predictive utility of adolescent IQ and parental SES remained stable for the most part. Yet, the combined effects of social-ecological and socioeconomic changes may have increased the predictive utility (that is, the regression weights) of adolescent GPA for educational, occupational, and health outcomes over time for individuals who were born in the 1980s. Theoretical implications concerning adult development, aging, and late life inequality are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record.
Recent research on proactive work behaviours (PWBs) pointed out that these behaviours can have negative consequences for the proactive individual. We add to this perspective by showing that PWBs may be a source of strain at work and result in elevated time pressure. Challenging the view of time pressure as a challenge stressor, we hypothesize that over the course of work weeks, time pressure will result in less (rather than more) PWB. We investigate these reciprocal effects as within-person, week-level fluctuations of time pressure and PWB based on experience sampling data (N = 52 participants, k = 274 observations). Over the course of three consecutive work weeks, results show a positive lagged effect of PWB in the first week on experiencing time pressure in the second week; in turn, time pressure in the second week had a negative lagged effect on PWB in the third week. Results further suggest that PWB is lowest in work weeks of low time pressure when following a week of high time pressure, indicating a conservation of resources interpretation of the results.
Research on narcissism continues to develop at a rapid pace. Yet, researchers from different disciplines are still divided over whether narcissists are good versus bad leaders. On the one hand, narcissists' bright qualities (e.g., charisma) are associated with positive outcomes at different levels of analysis from subordinates, to peers, and the organization as a whole. On the other hand, however, narcissists' dark qualities (e.g., entitlement) are associated with a number of counterproductive work behaviors, causing organizations to falter. The present article adds to and extends the traditional good-versus-bad debate about narcissistic leadership and pursues three goals: (a) to critically review the literature on narcissistic leaders and their behaviors in the workplace, (b) to provide tangible recommendations for how to best assess, select, and develop narcissistic leaders, and (c) to highlight future directions and ongoing challenges ahead in the field of narcissistic leadership.
The effects of potentially real and hypothetical rewards on effort discounting in a student sample
(2019)
The primary aim of the present study was to investigate the functional form of discounting of monetary rewards by physical effort in potentially real and hypothetical contexts. Individuals (N = 142) completed the assessments with hypothetical and potentially real effort discounting tasks, and their hypothetical or potentially real consequences. The data obtained from the experiment reported here suggest that hypothetical conditions yield patterns of discounting that mirror those for potentially real effort tasks and outcomes. However, this finding also leaves open the possibility that the degree of discounting may change together with the repeated exposure to the consequences of decisions, or the counterbalanced order of hypothetical and potentially real tasks.
Southeast Asia is one of the most dynamic regions in the world. It is experiencing rapid socioeconomic change that may influence the level of sexual aggression, but data on the scale of sexual aggression in the region remain sparse. The aim of the present article was to systematically review the findings of studies available in English on the prevalence of self-reported sexual aggression and victimization among women and men above the age of 12 years in the 11 countries of Southeast Asia (Brunei, Cambodia, East Timor, Indonesia, Laos, Malaysia, Myanmar, the Philippines, Singapore, Thailand, and Vietnam). Based on four scientific databases, the search engine Google, Opengrey database, and reference checking, 49 studies were found on sexual victimization. Of those, 32 included only women. Self-reported perpetration was assessed by only three studies and included all-male samples. Prevalence rates varied widely across studies but showed that sexual victimization was widespread among different social groups, irrespective of sex and sexual orientation. Methodological heterogeneity, lack of representativeness of samples, imbalance of information available by country, missing information within studies, and cultural differences hampered the comparability between and within countries. There is a need for operationalizations that specifically address sexual aggression occurring after the age of consent, based on detailed behavioral descriptions of unwanted sexual experiences and allied to a qualitative approach with cultural sensitivity. Data on sexual aggression in conflict settings and in human trafficking are also limited. Recommendations for future research are presented in the discussion.
The present study explored the age-related changes of eye movement control in reading-that is, where to send the eyes and when to move them. Different orthographies present readers with somewhat different problems to solve, and this might, in turn, be reflected in different patterns of development of reading skill. Participants of different developmental levels (Grade 3, N = 30; Grade 5, N = 27 and adults, N = 27) were instructed to read sentences for comprehension while their eye movements were recorded. Contrary to previous findings that have been well documented indicating early maturation of saccade generation in English, current results showed that saccade generation among Chinese readers was still under development at Grade 5, although immediate lexical processing was relatively well-established. The distinct age-related changes in eye movements are attributable to certain linguistic properties of Chinese including the lack of interword spaces and word boundary uncertainty. The present study offers an example of how human eye movement adapts to the orthographic environment.
Editorial
(2019)
Despite the fact that grade retention is now seen as controversial in many quarters, it remains common practice in numerous countries. Previous research on the effects of grade retention on student development has, however, generated ambiguous results, particularly in terms of motivational outcomes. This ambiguity has been attributed in part to a lack of high-quality studies including a longitudinal design, a suitable comparison group, and adequate statistical control of preretention differences. Based on longitudinal data of N = 3,288 German students over 3 years of secondary school, we examined differences in their academic self-concept, scholarly interests, learning motivation, and achievement motivation between those being retained in the 6th grade (n = 61) and those of the same age being promoted annually. To account for confounding variables, we applied full propensity score matching on baseline measures of the dependent variables, as well as various other covariates that have been found to be associated with the risk of retention (e.g., cognitive ability, academic performance, and family background variables). Results reveal a steep decline in students’ academic self-concept, interests, and learning motivation during the last months spent in the original class, just before retention. For those measures that were available, negative effects were still partly significant after 1 year, but had diminished 2 years after grade retention. Contrary to predictions suggested by the big-fish-little-pond effect, we found no positive effects of retention on students’ academic self-concept.
Chronic stress and emotion: Differential effects on attentional processing and recognition memory
(2019)
Previous research indicates that acute stress around the time of learning facilitates attention and memory for emotionally salient information. Despite accumulating evidence for these acute stress effects, less is known about the role of chronic stress. In the present study, we therefore tested emotional and neutral scene processing and later recognition memory in female participants using hair cortisol concentrations as a biological marker for chronic stress. Event-related potentials recorded during picture viewing indicated enhanced late positive potentials (LPPs) for emotional, relative to neutral contents. These brain potentials varied as a function of long-term hair cortisol levels: hair-cortisol levels were positively related to overall LPP amplitudes. Results from recognition memory testing one week after encoding revealed better memory for emotional relative to neutral scenes. Hair-cortisol levels, however, were related to poorer memory accuracy. Taken together, our results indicate that chronic stress enhanced attentional processing during encoding of new stimuli and impaired later recognition memory. Results are discussed with regard to putatively opposite effects of chronic stress on certain brain regions (e.g., amygdala and hippocampus).
To date, few studies have focused on perceived overprotection from the perspective of people with dementia (PwD). In the present examination, the association of perceived overprotection in PwD is examined as an autonomy-restricting factor and thus negative for their mental well-being. Cross-sectional data from the prospective DYADEM study of 82 patient/partner dyads (mean age = 74.26) were used to investigate the association between overprotection, perceived stress, depression, and quality of life (QoL). The analyses show that an overprotective contact style with PwD has a significant positive association with stress and depression, and has a negative association with QoL. The results emphasize the importance of avoiding an overprotective care style and supporting patient autonomy.
Cross-education has been extensively investigated with adults. Adult studies report asymmetrical cross-education adaptations predominately after dominant limb training. The objective of the study was to examine unilateral leg press (LP) training of the dominant or nondominant leg on contralateral and ipsilateral strength and balance measures. Forty-two youth (10-13 years) were placed (random allocation) into a dominant (n = 15) or nondominant (n = 14) leg press training group or nontraining control (n = 13). Experimental groups trained 3 times per week for 8 weeks and were tested pre-/post-training for ipsilateral and contralateral 1-repetition maximum (RM) horizontal LP, maximum voluntary isometric contraction (MVIC) of knee extensors (KE) and flexors (KF), countermovement jump (CMJ), triple hop test (THT), MVIC strength of elbow flexors (EF) and handgrip, as well as the stork and Y balance tests. Both dominant and nondominant LP training significantly (p < 0.05) increased both ipsilateral and contralateral lower body strength (LP 1RM (dominant: 59.6%-81.8%; nondominant: 59.5%-96.3%), KE MVIC (dominant: 12.4%-18.3%; nondominant: 8.6%-18.6%), KF MVIC (dominant: 7.9%-22.3%; nondominant: nonsignificant-3.8%), and power (CMJ: dominant: 11.1%-18.1%; nondominant: 7.7%-16.6%)). The exception was that nondominant LP training demonstrated a nonsignificant change with the contralateral KF MVIC. Other significant improvements were with nondominant LP training on ipsilateral EF 1RM (6.2%) and THT (9.6%). There were no significant changes with EF and handgrip MVIC. The contralateral leg stork balance test was impaired following dominant LP training. KF MVIC exhibited the only significant relative post-training to pretraining (post-test/pre-test) ratio differences between dominant versus nondominant LP cross-education training effects. In conclusion, children exhibit symmetrical cross-education or global training adaptations with unilateral training of dominant or nondominant upper leg.
Academic Achievement in Math and Foreign Language: Individual Characteristics and Gender Stereotypes
(2019)
The study aims to investigate the contribution of individual characteristics and gender stereotypes of secondary school pupils in academic achievement in math and foreign language. A sample of pupils reported in 6th and 7th grades (three waves) their self-perceived ability and motivation for math and foreign language learning, math and foreign language gender stereotypes, implicit theories of intelligence; academic achievement (grades) in math in foreign language is also included in the analysis. Regression analysis and Mann Whitney U Test demonstrated that self-perceived ability in math and incremental theory of intelligence are significant predictors for academic achievement in maths. Boys are characterised by higher level of self-perceived abilities and motivation for learning math. However, academic achievement of girls in maths and foreign languages are higher as compared to boys. Pupils learning two foreign languages demonstrate higher level of self-perceived abilities for motivation and learning math and less expressed gender stereotypes about success in learning math and foreign languages. Study findings can be used for designing educational programmes for mathematics and foreign languages, professional development and finding solutions for individualized approach in school education.
Recent research indicates that non- invasive stimulation of the afferent auricular vagal nerve (tVNS) may modulate various cognitive and affec-tive functions, likely via activation of the locus coeruleus- norepinephrine (LC- NE) system. In a series of ERP studies we found that the attention- related P300 component is enhanced during continuous vagal stimula-tion, compared to sham, which is also related to increased salivary alpha amylase levels (a putative indirect marker for central NE activation). In another study, we investigated the effect of continuous tVNS on the late positive potential (LPP), an electrophysiological index for motivated atten-tion toward emotionally evocative cues, and the effects of tVNS on later recognition memory (1- week delay). Here, vagal stimulation prompted earlier LPP differences (300- 500 ms) between unpleasant and neutral scenes. During retrieval, vagal stimulation significantly improved memory performance for unpleasant, but not neutral pictures, compared to sham stimulation, which was also related to enhanced salivary alpha amylase levels. In line, unpleasant images encoded under tVNS compared to sham stimulation also produced enhanced ERP old/new differences (500- 800 ms) during retrieval indicating better recollection. Taken together, our studies suggest that tVNS facilitates attention, learning and episodic memory, likely via afferent projections to the arousal- modulated LC- NE system. We will, however, also show data that point to critical stimulation parameters (likely duration and frequency) that need to be considered when applying tVNS
Der demografische Wandel wird nicht nur mit einer rasanten Zunahme der Hochaltrigen einhergehen [1], was für die gerontopsychiatrische Versorgung aufgrund der altersassoziierten Inzidenzraten in erster Linie eine Zunahme an Demenzerkrankungen und Patienten mit Multimorbidität und Gebrechlichkeit bedeutet [2], sondern auch mit einer Zunahme jüngerer alter Menschen vom 65. bis 75. Lebensjahr, was für die Gerontopsychiatrie eine Zunahme der Patienten mit Abhängigkeitserkrankungen, Erkrankungen aus dem schizophrenen Formenkreis und affektiven Erkrankungen bedeutet. Soziale Faktoren werden hier mehr und mehr eine zentrale Rolle spielen, da neben der Qualität der medizinischen Versorgung insbesondere die individuelle soziale Situation der Patienten mit einer erhöhten Morbidität und Mortalität einhergehen wird [3].
Wenn in der Medizin vom demografischen Wandel gesprochen wird [1], wird zumeist von einer rasanten Zunahme der Hochaltrigen gesprochen, bei denen aufgrund der differenziellen altersassoziierten Inzidenzraten in erster Linie eine Zunahme an Demenzerkrankungen, kardiovaskulären Erkrankungen, Krebserkrankungen und allgemeiner Multimorbidität und Gebrechlichkeit zu erwarten ist [2]. Dies ist unstrittig richtig, aber nur ein Teil der Folgen des demografischen Wandels für die psychiatrische Versorgung. Diese muss weiterhin die gesamte adulte Lebensspanne im Blick haben, da sonst Versorgungsengpässe bei ohnehin vulnerablen Patienten verstärkt werden, mit Folgen für die Morbidität und Mortalität auf Bevölkerungsebene [3].
IntroductionWhile physical activity (PA) can play an important role in the treatment of mental disorders (MD), large proportions of patients with MD do not meet PA recommendations. The aim of this trial was to evaluate whether structured psychological intervention (MoVo-LISA) is effective in helping outpatients with MD to increase their level of PA. As active control group (CG) we modified MoVo-LISA to target healthy diet behavior.MethodsN=83 outpatients with MD (F1-F4) were randomized to the two conditions. PA (self-report and accelerometry), dietary behavior, social-cognitive determinants of health behavior change, psychiatric symptoms and health-related quality of life were assessed at baseline, 1 and 12 weeks after the intervention.ResultsSignificant time*group interaction effects for objectively measured PA, dietary behavior and fruit and vegetable consumption indicated differential effects of the interventions on these outcomes. PA increased in the MoVo-LISA group (IG) from baseline to follow-up while it decreased in CG. IG showed a significant higher level of objectively measured PA at follow-up compared to CG. Dietary behavior and fruit and vegetable consumption significantly increased from baseline to follow-up in CG, but not IG. IG showed a significant increase in some, but not all social cognitive determinants of health behavior change.ConclusionsMoVo-LISA is effective in helping outpatients with MD to increase their level of PA in short- and mid-term. The used intervention strategies are effective for the promotion of healthy diet in patients with MD as well.
Communication with young children is often multimodal in nature, involving, for example, language and actions. The simultaneous presentation of information from both domains may boost language learning by highlighting the connection between an object and a word, owing to temporal overlap in the presentation of multimodal input. However, the overlap is not merely temporal but can also covary in the extent to which particular actions co-occur with particular words and objects, e.g. carers typically produce a hopping action when talking about rabbits and a snapping action for crocodiles. The frequency with which actions and words co-occurs in the presence of the referents of these words may also impact young children’s word learning. We, therefore, examined the extent to which consistency in the co-occurrence of particular actions and words impacted children’s learning of novel word–object associations. Children (18 months, 30 months and 36–48 months) and adults were presented with two novel objects and heard their novel labels while different actions were performed on these objects, such that the particular actions and word–object pairings always co-occurred (Consistent group) or varied across trials (Inconsistent group). At test, participants saw both objects and heard one of the labels to examine whether participants recognized the target object upon hearing its label. Growth curve models revealed that 18-month-olds did not learn words for objects in either condition, and 30-month-old and 36- to 48-month-old children learned words for objects only in the Consistent condition, in contrast to adults who learned words for objects independent of the actions presented. Thus, consistency in the multimodal input influenced word learning in early childhood but not in adulthood. In terms of a dynamic systems account of word learning, our study shows how multimodal learning settings interact with the child’s perceptual abilities to shape the learning experience.
Objective This prospective study explored bidirectional associations between attachment quality towards mother, father, and peers and disordered eating among a large population-based sample of boys and girls in the transition from preadolescence to adolescence. Specifically, we examined whether insecure attachment relationships emerged as a risk factor for or as an outcome of disordered eating. Method A population-based sample of 904 adolescent girls and boys was assessed four times, at baseline (T1; M-age = 10.8 years) and at 2-, 4-, and 6-year follow-up (T2, T3, and T4). Prospective data were analyzed using cross-lagged panel models for each attachment figure (i.e., mother, father, peers) in a multigroup design to compare genders. Results Better attachment to the mother led to less pronounced disturbed eating in girls across the entire age range and in boys across two time periods. In girls, more pronounced disordered eating at T3 predicted worse attachment to the mother at T4 and better attachment to the father at T1 predicted less disturbed eating at T2. In boys, disordered eating at T1 predicted better attachment to the father at T2. Concerning peer attachment, better attachment at T1 predicted disordered eating at T2, in boys only. No other significant cross-lagged effects emerged. Discussion These findings highlight the differential and gender-specific contribution of attachment figures to the development of disordered eating in adolescence. Programs aimed at improving communication and trust in the relationship with parents might be promising in the prevention of disordered eating and the subsequent deterioration of parent-child attachment relationships.
A recent extension of the stressor-detachment model holds that the path running from job stressors via psychological detachment to impairment of well-being is moderated by both personal and job resources (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2015). The aim of the present study was to test this proposition by investigating the moderating role of one personal resource and one job resource (i.e., coworker social support and general self-efficacy, respectively) on the linkage between different job stressors (i.e., workload and role ambiguity), detachment, and well-being. Hypotheses were tested with structural equation modeling using data from a representative survey of the German workforce (N = 3,937 employees, M-age = 46.5 years, 47.5% women). In agreement with previous findings, the results showed that psychological detachment mediated the negative effects of job stressors on well-being. Social support from coworkers buffered the mediation such that the conditional indirect effects of workload and role ambiguity on well-being via detachment were weaker at higher levels of support. General self-efficacy did not moderate the stressor-well-being linkage. These results imply that social support can be considered as a protective factor that helps employees maintain their well-being by alleviating the negative effects of job stressors on their ability to switch off mentally from work.