• Deutsch

University Logo

  • Home
  • Search
  • Browse
  • Submit
  • Sitemap
Schließen

Refine

Has Fulltext

  • no (165)
  • yes (121)

Author

  • Granacher, Urs (68)
  • Fischer, Martin H. (22)
  • Salzwedel, Annett (19)
  • Völler, Heinz (17)
  • Mayer, Frank (16)
  • Rapp, Michael Armin (16)
  • Wegscheider, Karl (12)
  • Zouhal, Hassane (12)
  • Muehlbauer, Thomas (11)
  • Bittmann, Frank N. (10)
+ more

Year of publication

  • 2020 (43)
  • 2019 (51)
  • 2018 (29)
  • 2017 (37)
  • 2016 (24)
  • 2015 (45)
  • 2014 (23)
  • 2013 (9)
  • 2012 (10)
  • 2011 (3)
+ more

Document Type

  • Article (148)
  • Postprint (103)
  • Doctoral Thesis (12)
  • Monograph/Edited Volume (7)
  • Conference Proceeding (6)
  • Review (5)
  • Preprint (3)
  • Habilitation (1)
  • Part of Periodical (1)

Language

  • English (273)
  • German (13)

Is part of the Bibliography

  • yes (275)
  • no (11)

Keywords

  • working memory (12)
  • fMRI (11)
  • football (10)
  • embodied cognition (8)
  • language acquisition (8)
  • neuroimaging (8)
  • German (7)
  • exercise (7)
  • aging (6)
  • locality (6)
  • performance (6)
  • physiology (6)
  • training load (6)
  • young athletes (6)
  • Cardiac rehabilitation (5)
  • individual differences (5)
  • insula (5)
  • interoceptive awareness (5)
  • major depressive disorder (5)
  • numerical cognition (5)
  • sentence processing (5)
  • Adaptive Force (4)
  • Children (4)
  • Mechanotendography (4)
  • Predictors (4)
  • Spanish (4)
  • activation (4)
  • adolescents (4)
  • childhood (4)
  • emotion (4)
  • exercise therapy (4)
  • expectation (4)
  • eye movements (4)
  • fatigue (4)
  • injury (4)
  • injury risk (4)
  • language (4)
  • mental arithmetic (4)
  • operational momentum (4)
  • osteoporosis (4)
  • rate of perceived exertion (4)
  • recovery (4)
  • resistance training (4)
  • rolling averages (4)
  • sentence comprehension (4)
  • weighted moving averages (4)
  • working memory capacity (4)
  • youth (4)
  • ACT-R (3)
  • Exercise (3)
  • Exercise therapy (3)
  • Eye movements (3)
  • Randomized controlled trial (3)
  • SNARC (3)
  • TAVI (3)
  • Working memory (3)
  • balance (3)
  • cardiac rehabilitation (3)
  • doping (3)
  • ground reaction force (3)
  • interoception (3)
  • meta-analysis (3)
  • physical fitness (3)
  • physical performance (3)
  • plyometric training (3)
  • relative clauses (3)
  • remission (3)
  • self-paced reading (3)
  • speech perception (3)
  • training (3)
  • AUD (2)
  • Achilles and patellar tendon (2)
  • Acoustic Analysis (2)
  • Aftercare (2)
  • Amygdala (2)
  • Anti-doping guideline (2)
  • Anti-doping program (2)
  • Appearance (2)
  • Arbeitsgedächtnis (2)
  • Attrition (2)
  • Back pain diagnosis (2)
  • Back pain prognosis (2)
  • Blickbewegungen (2)
  • Broca’s aphasia (2)
  • Cardiovascular diseases (2)
  • Catalan VCV Sequences (2)
  • Child (2)
  • Clinical psychology (2)
  • Clinical supervision (2)
  • Closure Positive Shift (CPS) (2)
  • Cognition (2)
  • Cognitive impairment (2)
  • Cost-effectiveness (2)
  • Cross-sectoral care (2)
  • DLT (2)
  • Decision making (2)
  • Delirium prevention (2)
  • Dementia (2)
  • Diary study (2)
  • Doping (2)
  • Education (2)
  • Elective surgery (2)
  • Elite sports schools (2)
  • English (2)
  • Event-related Potentials (ERP) (2)
  • Evidence-based psychotherapy (2)
  • Experience sampling method (2)
  • Facial Expressions (2)
  • Finger counting (2)
  • Frailty (2)
  • Games (2)
  • Geriatric rehabilitation (2)
  • Holding isometric muscle action (2)
  • Home-based (2)
  • Iambic/Trochaic Law (2)
  • Information (2)
  • Learning (2)
  • Lingual Coarticulation (2)
  • Locus Equations (2)
  • Mechanomyography (2)
  • Mental number line (2)
  • Metaanalysis (2)
  • Music cognition (2)
  • Obesity (2)
  • Older patients (2)
  • Outcome measures (2)
  • PROGRESS/TRIPOD (2)
  • Pain screening (2)
  • Perception (2)
  • Persian (2)
  • Postoperative cognitive dysfunction (2)
  • Prediction of disability/intensity (2)
  • Psycholinguistik (2)
  • Psychotherapeutic competencies (2)
  • Psychotherapy research (2)
  • Pushing isometric muscle action (2)
  • Quality of life (2)
  • Recognition Memory (2)
  • Recollection (2)
  • Regulatory focus (2)
  • Resistance (2)
  • Resistance training (2)
  • Role-playing (2)
  • SEMG-pattern (2)
  • SNARC effect (2)
  • Satzverarbeitung (2)
  • Scale development (2)
  • Self-stigmatization (2)
  • Simulated patients (2)
  • Speech Motor Control (2)
  • Spracherwerb (2)
  • Standardized patients (2)
  • State and trait measurement (2)
  • Stretch-shortening cycle (2)
  • Stroop effect (2)
  • Supervision (2)
  • Swimming performance (2)
  • Systematic review (2)
  • TMS (2)
  • Telerehabilitation (2)
  • Total hip replacement (2)
  • Total knee replacement (2)
  • Treatment pathways (2)
  • Trust (2)
  • Trustworthiness (2)
  • Two forms of isometric muscle action (2)
  • Ultrasound (2)
  • Video games (2)
  • Vocal-Tract (2)
  • Weight (2)
  • Weight bias internalization (2)
  • Weight management trial (2)
  • Yellow flags (2)
  • Young swimmers (2)
  • abstract concepts (2)
  • accommodation (2)
  • action language (2)
  • action processing (2)
  • action segmentation (2)
  • action words (2)
  • acute chronic workload ratio (2)
  • adaptation to external force impact (2)
  • additive particles (2)
  • adolescent sport (2)
  • aftercare (2)
  • aggression (2)
  • agility (2)
  • aging brain (2)
  • alcohol consumption (2)
  • alexithymia (2)
  • alternatives (2)
  • amateur soccer players (2)
  • ambiguities (2)
  • anaphoricity (2)
  • anger (2)
  • animacy (2)
  • ankles (2)
  • antilocality (2)
  • approach (2)
  • arithmetic (2)
  • athletic performance (2)
  • attention (2)
  • attitude (2)
  • automatic facial expression analysis (2)
  • avoidance (2)
  • back pain (2)
  • balance strategy (2)
  • balance training (2)
  • behavioral weight loss (2)
  • bilingualism (2)
  • biological maturation (2)
  • biomechanics (2)
  • blood (2)
  • body density (2)
  • body limbs (2)
  • bone mineral density (2)
  • bone pathologies (2)
  • bone–brain–nervous system interactions (2)
  • break interventions (2)
  • broadband and narrowband dimensions of behavior (2)
  • calcaneal eversion (2)
  • canoe racing (2)
  • carcinoma (2)
  • carryover effects (2)
  • change (2)
  • change of direction (2)
  • change-of-direction speed (2)
  • chemo-toxicity (2)
  • clauses (2)
  • clinical pain research (2)
  • coarticulation (2)
  • cognition (2)
  • cognitive decline (2)
  • cognitive linguistics (2)
  • cognitive module (2)
  • cognitive psychology (2)
  • cognitive resources (2)
  • cognitive training (2)
  • cognitive-postural dual task (2)
  • complaints (2)
  • complex predicates (2)
  • computer games (2)
  • concurrent training (2)
  • confidence (2)
  • contractions (2)
  • controlled trial (2)
  • core strength (2)
  • cross-domain priming (2)
  • cruciate ligament injury (2)
  • deep learning (2)
  • derivation (2)
  • developmental dyslexia (2)
  • discourse (2)
  • distributed processing (2)
  • drop jump (2)
  • dual-task (2)
  • early sport specialization (2)
  • electromyography (2)
  • elite athletes (2)
  • embodiment (2)
  • emerging adults (2)
  • endocrine pathways (2)
  • entropy (2)
  • epidemiology (2)
  • episodic memory (2)
  • executive function (2)
  • executive functions (2)
  • exercise program (2)
  • experimental data (2)
  • external training load (2)
  • eye tracking (2)
  • eye-tracking (2)
  • eyedness (2)
  • eyes (2)
  • feet (2)
  • figurative language (2)
  • footedness (2)
  • freedom restriction (2)
  • gastrointestinal cancer (2)
  • gestural organization (2)
  • goal frames (2)
  • hamstring injuries (2)
  • hamstring muscles (2)
  • hand (2)
  • heart rate variability (2)
  • hematocrit (2)
  • hemispheric asymmetry (2)
  • heuristics and biases (2)
  • high-intensity interval training (2)
  • hip (2)
  • home-based (2)
  • hopelessness (2)
  • hormones (2)
  • human performance (2)
  • humans (2)
  • hypothesis (2)
  • idiom (2)
  • impact on pre-activated Achilles tendon (2)
  • inclusive education (2)
  • information integration (2)
  • input frequency (2)
  • interference (2)
  • internalizing behavior (2)
  • isometric eccentric force (2)
  • jump performance (2)
  • jump training (2)
  • jumping (2)
  • kinematic boundary cues (2)
  • knee osteoarthritis (2)
  • knees (2)
  • latency (2)
  • laterality (2)
  • lexicon size (2)
  • life events (2)
  • limb overuse conditions (2)
  • lingering misinterpretation (2)
  • literacy acquisition (2)
  • long-term (2)
  • longitudinal study (2)
  • low-back-pain (2)
  • lower-extremity kinematics (2)
  • magnetoencephalography (2)
  • material fatigue (2)
  • maturity (2)
  • mechanical tendinous oscillations (2)
  • medial longitudinal arch (2)
  • memory (2)
  • mental number line (2)
  • mental number line (MNL) (2)
  • mental simulation (2)
  • metaphor (2)
  • methodology (2)
  • mind wandering (2)
  • modality (2)
  • modality compatibility (2)
  • monitoring (2)
  • morphological decomposition (2)
  • morphological errors (2)
  • motivation (2)
  • motor control (2)
  • motor resonance (2)
  • motor skills (2)
  • motor system (2)
  • motor unit synchronization (2)
  • mozart effect (2)
  • muscle (2)
  • muscle action (2)
  • muscle activation (2)
  • muscle coactivation (2)
  • muscle fitness (2)
  • muscle power (2)
  • muscular activity (2)
  • music cognition (2)
  • music information retrieval (2)
  • music perception (2)
  • musicality (2)
  • negative expectation (2)
  • neuroendocrine (2)
  • neuromuscular (2)
  • neuromuscular pre-activation (2)
  • neuroplasticity (2)
  • newborns (2)
  • non-athletes (2)
  • number (2)
  • oarsmen (2)
  • obesity (2)
  • occupational prognosis (2)
  • on-water performance (2)
  • overreaching (2)
  • overtraining (2)
  • overuse injuries (2)
  • passive stretching (2)
  • peak torque (2)
  • pelvic alignment (2)
  • physical fitness expertise (2)
  • plyometrics (2)
  • postural control (2)
  • postural stability (2)
  • power (2)
  • power improvement (2)
  • pre-activity (2)
  • prediction (2)
  • prefixes (2)
  • presupposition (2)
  • prevalence information (2)
  • primary school (2)
  • proactive aggression (2)
  • processing (2)
  • prosodic boundary cues (2)
  • prosody processing (2)
  • prototype-willingness-model (2)
  • psycholinguistics (2)
  • psychosocial stress (2)
  • quality management (2)
  • quality of life (2)
  • race time (2)
  • randomized-controlled-trial (2)
  • range of motion (2)
  • reactance (2)
  • reactive aggression (2)
  • recognition (2)
  • recollection (2)
  • rehabilitation (2)
  • relational aggression (2)
  • reliability (2)
  • replication (2)
  • representation learning (2)
  • responses (2)
  • return to work (2)
  • rhythm perception (2)
  • rhythmic grouping (2)
  • risk factors (2)
  • risk-factors (2)
  • rotation (2)
  • running (2)
  • sACC (2)
  • salivary alpha-amylase (2)
  • schedule (2)
  • scholastic demands (2)
  • self threat (2)
  • sex-differences (2)
  • skeletal joints (2)
  • social inclusion (2)
  • social reactivity (2)
  • sociometric neglect (2)
  • sociometric status (2)
  • solid tumor (2)
  • sonography (2)
  • spatial frequency (SF) (2)
  • spatial-numerical associations (2)
  • special educational needs (2)
  • specificity (2)
  • speech kinematics (2)
  • speech motor control (2)
  • speech segmentation (2)
  • spelling (2)
  • sport-specific performance (2)
  • sports (2)
  • sprinting (2)
  • standing position (2)
  • strength measurement system (2)
  • stretch-shortening cycle (2)
  • surprisal (2)
  • symptoms (2)
  • syntax (2)
  • systems (2)
  • talent (2)
  • task (2)
  • team handball (2)
  • team sport (2)
  • telerehabilitation (2)
  • temporal frequency (2)
  • time (2)
  • total hip replacement (2)
  • total knee replacement (2)
  • training adaptation (2)
  • training intensity (2)
  • training-program (2)
  • transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (2)
  • transfer (2)
  • trunk (2)
  • ultrasound imaging (2)
  • validity (2)
  • variability (2)
  • verb-phrase ellipsis (2)
  • vowels (2)
  • women (2)
  • words (2)
  • work capacity (2)
  • yellow flags (2)
  • youth football (2)
  • 1-to-1 Correspondence (1)
  • Acquired dysgraphia (1)
  • Adaptive control (1)
  • Adolescents (1)
  • Age of acquisition (1)
  • Aggression (1)
  • Agrammatismus (1)
  • Aortic valve (1)
  • Aspekt (1)
  • Assembly task (1)
  • Attention (1)
  • Aufsatzsammlung (1)
  • Background (1)
  • Balance (1)
  • Blickbewegungsmessung (1)
  • Bulgarian (1)
  • Bulgarisch (1)
  • Cardinality (1)
  • Cardiovascular risk factors (1)
  • Category verification (1)
  • Chinese (1)
  • Chinesisch (1)
  • Chronic heart failure (CHF) (1)
  • Clinical study (1)
  • Cloze predictability (1)
  • Co-occurrence probability (1)
  • Competitive sport (1)
  • Computational modeling (1)
  • Contrast (1)
  • Countermovement jump (1)
  • Covert orienting (1)
  • Crossover fatigue (1)
  • Cultural intelligence (1)
  • Desensibilisierung (1)
  • Deutsch (1)
  • Disengagement (1)
  • EKP (1)
  • ERP (1)
  • Embodied cognition (1)
  • Endocrine (1)
  • Engagement (1)
  • Entscheidungen (1)
  • Event-related potentials (1)
  • Exercise tests (1)
  • Eyetracking (1)
  • Fall risk (1)
  • Fall risk assessment (1)
  • Finger-based numerical representations (1)
  • Finger-digit mapping (1)
  • Fixation duration (1)
  • Fixational eye movements (1)
  • Focus (1)
  • Force (1)
  • Forced drop-out (1)
  • Frail elderly (1)
  • Future (1)
  • Generalisation (1)
  • Genetics (1)
  • Givenness (1)
  • Grundschüler/innen (1)
  • Health economics (1)
  • Health promotion (1)
  • Heart (1)
  • Hirnentwicklung (1)
  • Home telemonitoring (1)
  • Hungarian (1)
  • Hypercholesterolemia (1)
  • Hypertension (1)
  • Ibero-Romance (1)
  • Incremental Cost-Effectiveness Ratio (ICER) (1)
  • Information structure (1)
  • Instability (1)
  • Intercultural competence (1)
  • Kognitionswissenschaften (1)
  • Komplementsatzverstehen (1)
  • Lateral jumps (1)
  • Lesen (1)
  • Lesespanne (1)
  • Lesestrategie (1)
  • Linguistik (1)
  • Load (1)
  • Mediengewalt (1)
  • Melatonin receptor type 2 (1)
  • Mental arithmetic (1)
  • Mental disorders (1)
  • Mental health (1)
  • Mental time line (1)
  • Mental time travel (1)
  • Metabolism (1)
  • Metaphors (1)
  • Microsaccades (1)
  • Mortality (1)
  • Multiple risk factor intervention (1)
  • Multisensory (1)
  • Muscle (1)
  • N400 (1)
  • Negation (1)
  • Nuclear accent (1)
  • Number processing (1)
  • Operational momentum (1)
  • Oral anticoagulation (1)
  • Oral anticoagulation therapy (1)
  • Ordinality (1)
  • Orienting (1)
  • Overt attention (1)
  • Partikelverben (1)
  • Past (1)
  • Patholinguistik (1)
  • Patient self-management (1)
  • Pointing (1)
  • Postural stability (1)
  • Power (1)
  • Preaktivierung (1)
  • Predictability (1)
  • Preventive therapy (1)
  • Prinzipal-Agent (1)
  • Prosodic phrasing (1)
  • Prosody (1)
  • Psychologie (1)
  • Psychology (1)
  • Raum (1)
  • Reading (1)
  • Reflex (1)
  • Reflexivpronomen (1)
  • Rehearsal (1)
  • Relativsätze (1)
  • Satzlesen (1)
  • Schiedsrichter (1)
  • School (1)
  • Semantic priming (1)
  • Semantic typicality (1)
  • Seniors (1)
  • Sequential instruction (1)
  • Situated cognition (1)
  • Skill (1)
  • Smoking (1)
  • Smoking cessation (1)
  • Spatial-temporal association (1)
  • Spelling (1)
  • Sportvereinsmitgliedschaft (1)
  • Sprachtherapie (1)
  • Sprechapraxie (1)
  • Telemedicine (1)
  • Theory of Mind (1)
  • Time course (1)
  • Trainer (1)
  • Transcatheter Aortic Valve Implantation (1)
  • Travel (1)
  • Treatment (1)
  • Unaccusativity (1)
  • Unergative verbs (1)
  • Urteilsverzerrung (1)
  • Videospiele (1)
  • Vitamin k-antagonists (1)
  • Vorhersagen (1)
  • Wohngegend (1)
  • Word frequency (1)
  • Worterkennung (1)
  • Zahl (1)
  • acoustic phonetics (1)
  • adherence (1)
  • adolescence (1)
  • adolescent (1)
  • agrammatism (1)
  • apraxia of speech (1)
  • aspect (1)
  • behavior (1)
  • bias (1)
  • bone (1)
  • brain development (1)
  • brain lesions (1)
  • bullying (1)
  • cardiorespiratory fitness (1)
  • child (1)
  • child language (1)
  • children (1)
  • cholinesterase inhibitors (1)
  • chronic kidney disease (1)
  • classroom climate (1)
  • climbing (1)
  • clinical study (1)
  • cognitive enhancement (1)
  • cognitive modeling (1)
  • cognitive sciences (1)
  • compatibility effect (1)
  • complementation (1)
  • complex sentence processing (1)
  • comprehension of complement sentences (1)
  • computer aided dosage (1)
  • conditioning stimulus (1)
  • conduction aphasia (1)
  • connectivity (1)
  • content-addressable memory (1)
  • control rates (1)
  • core (1)
  • cytochrome P450 17A1 (Cyp17A1) (1)
  • decannulation protocol (1)
  • decision making (1)
  • depression (1)
  • desensitization (1)
  • dilatational tracheostomy (1)
  • dimensional (1)
  • dimensional overlap (1)
  • disease severity (1)
  • dose-response relation (1)
  • dose-response relationship (1)
  • dual task (1)
  • dyslipidemia (1)
  • dysphagia (1)
  • early speech perception and later language performance (1)
  • elderly (1)
  • elite sport (1)
  • emotion recognition (1)
  • emotional expression (1)
  • exercise stress test (1)
  • expert system (1)
  • eye movement (1)
  • face morphing (1)
  • facial feedback (1)
  • family risk for SLI (1)
  • force (1)
  • frequency (1)
  • gait (1)
  • genetics (1)
  • gestures (1)
  • glomerular filtration rate (1)
  • heart (1)
  • hebrew (1)
  • human language processing (1)
  • hypertension (1)
  • impact loading (1)
  • implicit (1)
  • implizit (1)
  • incorporation (1)
  • indirect dependency (1)
  • individuelle Unterschiede (1)
  • inflection (1)
  • intervention (1)
  • joint Simon effect (1)
  • jump height/power (1)
  • kognitive Modellierung (1)
  • language network (1)
  • leg extensors (1)
  • lesion studies (1)
  • lexical databases (1)
  • lexicon (1)
  • lexikalische Datenbanken (1)
  • lifespan (1)
  • linguistic determinism (1)
  • linguistics (1)
  • linguistische Determinismushypothese (1)
  • lipids (1)
  • living area (1)
  • long distance wh-movement (1)
  • mathematical cognition (1)
  • maximal isometric torque (1)
  • media violence (1)
  • memantine (1)
  • menschliche Sprachverarbeitung (1)
  • morphology (1)
  • morphosyntax (1)
  • motorische Leistungsfähigkeit (1)
  • multimodal cardiac rehabilitation (1)
  • muscle strength (1)
  • musculature (1)
  • native dialects (1)
  • negation (1)
  • neuroenhancement (1)
  • non-fluent aphasia (1)
  • non-native dialects (1)
  • non-native speech perception (1)
  • numeracy training (1)
  • older adults (1)
  • outcome (1)
  • outcome measures (1)
  • parafoveal-on-foveal effects (1)
  • particle verbs (1)
  • past-tense (1)
  • patholinguistics (1)
  • performance enhancement (1)
  • phenprocoumon (1)
  • physical activity (1)
  • physical conditioning human (1)
  • physiologische Verfahren (1)
  • plasticity (1)
  • portuguese (1)
  • postural balance (1)
  • preactivation (1)
  • processing of auditory nonverbal stimuli (1)
  • psychophysiological measures (1)
  • quality indicator (1)
  • rate of torque development (1)
  • reading (1)
  • reading strategy (1)
  • reading-span (1)
  • referees (1)
  • referential coding (1)
  • reflexives (1)
  • registry (1)
  • rehabilitation outcome (1)
  • relational bullying (1)
  • respiration (1)
  • review (1)
  • reward anticipation (1)
  • reward system (1)
  • rhythmic discrimination (1)
  • risk factor (1)
  • rules (1)
  • school (1)
  • school-aged children (1)
  • selbstbestimmtes Lesen (1)
  • self-efficacy (1)
  • sensory input (1)
  • sentence reading (1)
  • sequential attention shifts (1)
  • shoulder girdle (1)
  • skipping costs/benefits (1)
  • soccer (1)
  • socioeconomic status (1)
  • sozioökonomischer Status (1)
  • spatial response coding (1)
  • speech and language therapy (1)
  • speech pathology (1)
  • speech production (1)
  • sports club participation (1)
  • static/dynamic postural control (1)
  • stoma button (1)
  • success (1)
  • syntactic expectation (1)
  • task difficulty (1)
  • theory of mind (1)
  • therapy (1)
  • therapy volume (1)
  • transdiagnostic (1)
  • ventral striatum (1)
  • verbal bullying (1)
  • verbal working memory (1)
  • verbales Arbeitsgedächtnis (1)
  • verteilte Verarbeitung (1)
  • video games (1)
  • warfarin (1)
  • wh-scope marker (1)
  • willingness to intervene (1)
  • word processing (1)
  • word recognition (1)
  • youth sports (1)
  • fixation duration (1)
- less

Institute

  • Strukturbereich Kognitionswissenschaften (286) (remove)

286 search hits

  • 1 to 20
  • BibTeX
  • CSV
  • RIS
  • XML
  • 10
  • 20
  • 50
  • 100

Sort by

  • Year
  • Year
  • Title
  • Title
  • Author
  • Author
A best practice fall prevention exercise program to improve balance, strength/power, and psychosocial health in older adults - study protocol for a randomized controlled trial (2013)
Gschwind, Yves J. ; Kressig, Reto W. ; Lacroix, Andre ; Muehlbauer, Thomas ; Pfenninger, Barbara ; Granacher, Urs
Background: With increasing age neuromuscular deficits (e.g., sarcopenia) may result in impaired physical performance and an increased risk for falls. Prominent intrinsic fall-risk factors are age-related decreases in balance and strength / power performance as well as cognitive decline. Additional studies are needed to develop specifically tailored exercise programs for older adults that can easily be implemented into clinical practice. Thus, the objective of the present trial is to assess the effects of a fall prevention program that was developed by an interdisciplinary expert panel on measures of balance, strength / power, body composition, cognition, psychosocial well-being, and falls self-efficacy in healthy older adults. Additionally, the time-related effects of detraining are tested. Methods/Design: Healthy old people (n = 54) between the age of 65 to 80 years will participate in this trial. The testing protocol comprises tests for the assessment of static / dynamic steady-state balance (i.e., Sharpened Romberg Test, instrumented gait analysis), proactive balance (i.e., Functional Reach Test; Timed Up and Go Test), reactive balance (i.e., perturbation test during bipedal stance; Push and Release Test), strength (i.e., hand grip strength test; Chair Stand Test), and power (i.e., Stair Climb Power Test; countermovement jump). Further, body composition will be analysed using a bioelectrical impedance analysis system. In addition, questionnaires for the assessment of psychosocial (i.e., World Health Organisation Quality of Life Assessment-Bref), cognitive (i.e., Mini Mental State Examination), and fall risk determinants (i.e., Fall Efficacy Scale -International) will be included in the study protocol. Participants will be randomized into two intervention groups or the control / waiting group. After baseline measures, participants in the intervention groups will conduct a 12-week balance and strength / power exercise intervention 3 times per week, with each training session lasting 30 min. (actual training time). One intervention group will complete an extensive supervised training program, while the other intervention group will complete a short version (` 3 times 3') that is home-based and controlled by weekly phone calls. Post-tests will be conducted right after the intervention period. Additionally, detraining effects will be measured 12 weeks after program cessation. The control group / waiting group will not participate in any specific intervention during the experimental period, but will receive the extensive supervised program after the experimental period. Discussion: It is expected that particularly the supervised combination of balance and strength / power training will improve performance in variables of balance, strength / power, body composition, cognitive function, psychosocial well-being, and falls self-efficacy of older adults. In addition, information regarding fall risk assessment, dose-response-relations, detraining effects, and supervision of training will be provided. Further, training-induced health-relevant changes, such as improved performance in activities of daily living, cognitive function, and quality of life, as well as a reduced risk for falls may help to lower costs in the health care system. Finally, practitioners, therapists, and instructors will be provided with a scientifically evaluated feasible, safe, and easy-to-administer exercise program for fall prevention.
A comparison of running kinetics in children with and without genu varus (2017)
Jafarnezhadgero, Amir Ali ; Shad, Morteza Madadi ; Majlesi, Mahdi ; Granacher, Urs
Introduction Varus knee alignment has been identified as a risk factor for the progression of medial knee osteoarthritis. However, the underlying mechanisms have not been elucidated yet in children. Thus, the aims of the present study were to examine differences in ground reaction forces, loading rate, impulses, and free moment values during running in children with and without genu varus. Methods Thirty-six boys aged 9–14 volunteered to participate in this study. They were divided in two age-matched groups (genu varus versus healthy controls). Body weight adjusted three dimensional kinetic data (Fx, Fy, Fz) were collected during running at preferred speed using two Kistler force plates for the dominant and non-dominant limb. Results Individuals with knee genu varus produced significantly higher (p = .01; d = 1.09; 95%) body weight adjusted ground reaction forces in the lateral direction (Fx) of the dominant limb compared to controls. On the non-dominant limb, genu varus patients showed significantly higher body weight adjusted ground reaction forces values in the lateral (p = .01; d = 1.08; 86%) and medial (p < .001; d = 1.55; 102%) directions (Fx). Further, genu varus patients demonstrated 55% and 36% greater body weight adjusted loading rates in the dominant (p < .001; d = 2.09) and non-dominant (p < .001; d = 1.02) leg, respectively. No significant between-group differences were observed for adjusted free moment values (p>.05). Discussion Higher mediolateral ground reaction forces and vertical loading rate amplitudes in boys with genu varus during running at preferred running speed may accelerate the development of progressive joint degeneration in terms of the age at knee osteoarthritis onset. Therefore, practitioners and therapists are advised to conduct balance and strength training programs to improve lower limb alignment and mediolateral control during dynamic movements.
A comparison of running kinetics in children with and without genu varus (2017)
Jafarnezhadgero, Amir Ali ; Shad, Morteza Madadi ; Majlesi, Mahdi ; Granacher, Urs
Introduction Varus knee alignment has been identified as a risk factor for the progression of medial knee osteoarthritis. However, the underlying mechanisms have not been elucidated yet in children. Thus, the aims of the present study were to examine differences in ground reaction forces, loading rate, impulses, and free moment values during running in children with and without genu varus. Methods Thirty-six boys aged 9–14 volunteered to participate in this study. They were divided in two age-matched groups (genu varus versus healthy controls). Body weight adjusted three dimensional kinetic data (Fx, Fy, Fz) were collected during running at preferred speed using two Kistler force plates for the dominant and non-dominant limb. Results Individuals with knee genu varus produced significantly higher (p = .01; d = 1.09; 95%) body weight adjusted ground reaction forces in the lateral direction (Fx) of the dominant limb compared to controls. On the non-dominant limb, genu varus patients showed significantly higher body weight adjusted ground reaction forces values in the lateral (p = .01; d = 1.08; 86%) and medial (p < .001; d = 1.55; 102%) directions (Fx). Further, genu varus patients demonstrated 55% and 36% greater body weight adjusted loading rates in the dominant (p < .001; d = 2.09) and non-dominant (p < .001; d = 1.02) leg, respectively. No significant between-group differences were observed for adjusted free moment values (p>.05). Discussion Higher mediolateral ground reaction forces and vertical loading rate amplitudes in boys with genu varus during running at preferred running speed may accelerate the development of progressive joint degeneration in terms of the age at knee osteoarthritis onset. Therefore, practitioners and therapists are advised to conduct balance and strength training programs to improve lower limb alignment and mediolateral control during dynamic movements.
A cross-dialectal acoustic comparison of vowels in Northern and Southern British English (2014)
Williams, Daniel ; Escudero, Paola
This study compares the duration and first two formants (F1 and F2) of 11 nominal monophthongs and five nominal diphthongs in Standard Southern British English (SSBE) and a Northern English dialect. F1 and F2 trajectories were fitted with parametric curves using the discrete cosine transform (DCT) and the zeroth DCT coefficient represented formant trajectory means and the first DCT coefficient represented the magnitude and direction of formant trajectory change to characterize vowel inherent spectral change (VISC). Cross-dialectal comparisons involving these measures revealed significant differences for the phonologically back monophthongs /D, , , u:/ and also /3z:/ and the diphthongs /eI, e, aI, I/. Most cross-dialectal differences are in zeroth DCT coefficients, suggesting formant trajectory means tend to characterize such differences, while first DCT coefficient differences were more numerous for diphthongs. With respect to VISC, the most striking differences are that /u:/is considerably more diphthongized in the Northern dialect and that the F2 trajectory of /e/proceeds in opposite directions in the two dialects. Cross-dialectal differences were found to be largely unaffected by the consonantal context in which the vowels were produced. The implications of the results are discussed in relation to VISC, consonantal context effects and speech perception. (c) 2014 Acoustical Society of America.
A Meta-Analysis to Determine Strength Training Related Dose-Response Relationships for Lower-Limb Muscle Power Development in Young Athletes (2018)
Slimani, Maamer ; Paravlic, Armin ; Granacher, Urs
It is well-documented that strength training (ST) improves measures of muscle strength in young athletes. Less is known on transfer effects of ST on proxies of muscle power and the underlying dose-response relationships. The objectives of this meta-analysis were to quantify the effects of ST on lower limb muscle power in young athletes and to provide dose-response relationships for ST modalities such as frequency, intensity, and volume. A systematic literature search of electronic databases identified 895 records. Studies were eligible for inclusion if (i) healthy trained children (girls aged 6–11 y, boys aged 6–13 y) or adolescents (girls aged 12–18 y, boys aged 14–18 y) were examined, (ii) ST was compared with an active control, and (iii) at least one proxy of muscle power [squat jump (SJ) and countermovement jump height (CMJ)] was reported. Weighted mean standardized mean differences (SMDwm) between subjects were calculated. Based on the findings from 15 statistically aggregated studies, ST produced significant but small effects on CMJ height (SMDwm = 0.65; 95% CI 0.34–0.96) and moderate effects on SJ height (SMDwm = 0.80; 95% CI 0.23–1.37). The sub-analyses revealed that the moderating variable expertise level (CMJ height: p = 0.06; SJ height: N/A) did not significantly influence ST-related effects on proxies of muscle power. “Age” and “sex” moderated ST effects on SJ (p = 0.005) and CMJ height (p = 0.03), respectively. With regard to the dose-response relationships, findings from the meta-regression showed that none of the included training modalities predicted ST effects on CMJ height. For SJ height, the meta-regression indicated that the training modality “training duration” significantly predicted the observed gains (p = 0.02), with longer training durations (>8 weeks) showing larger improvements. This meta-analysis clearly proved the general effectiveness of ST on lower-limb muscle power in young athletes, irrespective of the moderating variables. Dose-response analyses revealed that longer training durations (>8 weeks) are more effective to improve SJ height. No such training modalities were found for CMJ height. Thus, there appear to be other training modalities besides the ones that were included in our analyses that may have an effect on SJ and particularly CMJ height. ST monitoring through rating of perceived exertion, movement velocity or force-velocity profile could be promising monitoring tools for lower-limb muscle power development in young athletes.
A Meta-Analysis to Determine Strength Training Related Dose-Response Relationships for Lower-Limb Muscle Power Development in Young Athletes (2018)
Slimani, Maamer ; Paravlic, Armin ; Granacher, Urs
It is well-documented that strength training (ST) improves measures of muscle strength in young athletes. Less is known on transfer effects of ST on proxies of muscle power and the underlying dose-response relationships. The objectives of this meta-analysis were to quantify the effects of ST on lower limb muscle power in young athletes and to provide dose-response relationships for ST modalities such as frequency, intensity, and volume. A systematic literature search of electronic databases identified 895 records. Studies were eligible for inclusion if (i) healthy trained children (girls aged 6–11 y, boys aged 6–13 y) or adolescents (girls aged 12–18 y, boys aged 14–18 y) were examined, (ii) ST was compared with an active control, and (iii) at least one proxy of muscle power [squat jump (SJ) and countermovement jump height (CMJ)] was reported. Weighted mean standardized mean differences (SMDwm) between subjects were calculated. Based on the findings from 15 statistically aggregated studies, ST produced significant but small effects on CMJ height (SMDwm = 0.65; 95% CI 0.34–0.96) and moderate effects on SJ height (SMDwm = 0.80; 95% CI 0.23–1.37). The sub-analyses revealed that the moderating variable expertise level (CMJ height: p = 0.06; SJ height: N/A) did not significantly influence ST-related effects on proxies of muscle power. “Age” and “sex” moderated ST effects on SJ (p = 0.005) and CMJ height (p = 0.03), respectively. With regard to the dose-response relationships, findings from the meta-regression showed that none of the included training modalities predicted ST effects on CMJ height. For SJ height, the meta-regression indicated that the training modality “training duration” significantly predicted the observed gains (p = 0.02), with longer training durations (>8 weeks) showing larger improvements. This meta-analysis clearly proved the general effectiveness of ST on lower-limb muscle power in young athletes, irrespective of the moderating variables. Dose-response analyses revealed that longer training durations (>8 weeks) are more effective to improve SJ height. No such training modalities were found for CMJ height. Thus, there appear to be other training modalities besides the ones that were included in our analyses that may have an effect on SJ and particularly CMJ height. ST monitoring through rating of perceived exertion, movement velocity or force-velocity profile could be promising monitoring tools for lower-limb muscle power development in young athletes.
A multimodal measure of cultural intelligence for adolescents growing up in culturally diverse societies (2019)
Schwarzenthal, Miriam ; Juang, Linda P. ; Schachner, Maja K. ; van de Vijver, Fons J. R.
Adolescents growing up in culturally diverse societies need to develop intercultural competence. To better understand how to develop intercultural competence we need measures specifically relating to the everyday intercultural experiences of adolescents. However, few measures of intercultural competence are available for this target group. Based on the cultural intelligence (CQ) model (Earley & Ang, 2003), we developed a measure that combines a self-report questionnaire and situational judgment tests (SJTs). The latter comprise a brief description of intercultural situations, followed by questions asking the adolescents to interpret and provide a reaction to the situations. The reliability, factor structure, measurement equivalence, and validity of the new measure was tested in two samples of adolescents in culturally diverse regions in North Rhine-Westphalia (N = 631, 48% female, M-a(ge) = 13.69 years, SDage = 1.83) and Berlin (N = 1,335, 48% female, M-age = 14.69 years, SDage, = 0.74) in Germany. The self-report CQ scale showed good reliability and a four-dimensional factor structure with a higher-order CQ factor. The responses to the SJTs were coded based on a coding manual and the ratings loaded onto one factor. The measurement models showed metric to scalar measurement equivalence across immigrant background, gender, and grade. The CQ factor and the SJT factor were positively correlated with each other, as well as with related constructs such as openness, perspective-taking, and diversity beliefs. We conclude that the new measure offers a reliable and valid method to assess the intercultural competence of adolescents growing up in culturally diverse societies.
A Single Nucleotide Polymorphism near the CYP17A1 Gene Is Associated with Left Ventricular Mass in Hypertensive Patients under Pharmacotherapy (2015)
Huber, Matthias ; Lezius, Susanne ; Reibis, Rona Katharina ; Treszl, Andras ; Kujawinska, Dorota ; Jakob, Stefanie ; Wegscheider, Karl ; Völler, Heinz ; Kreutz, Reinhold
Cytochrome P450 17A1 (CYP17A1) catalyses the formation and metabolism of steroid hormones. They are involved in blood pressure (BP) regulation and in the pathogenesis of left ventricular hypertrophy. Therefore, altered function of CYP17A1 due to genetic variants may influence BP and left ventricular mass. Notably, genome wide association studies supported the role of this enzyme in BP control. Against this background, we investigated associations between single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in or nearby the CYP17A1 gene with BP and left ventricular mass in patients with arterial hypertension and associated cardiovascular organ damage treated according to guidelines. Patients (n = 1007, mean age 58.0 +/- 9.8 years, 83% men) with arterial hypertension and cardiac left ventricular ejection fraction (LVEF) 40% were enrolled in the study. Cardiac parameters of left ventricular mass, geometry and function were determined by echocardiography. The cohort comprised patients with coronary heart disease (n = 823; 81.7%) and myocardial infarction (n = 545; 54.1%) with a mean LVEF of 59.9% +/- 9.3%. The mean left ventricular mass index (LVMI) was 52.1 +/- 21.2 g/m(2.7) and 485 (48.2%) patients had left ventricular hypertrophy. There was no significant association of any investigated SNP (rs619824, rs743572, rs1004467, rs11191548, rs17115100) with mean 24 h systolic or diastolic BP. However, carriers of the rs11191548 C allele demonstrated a 7% increase in LVMI (95% CI: 1%-12%, p = 0.017) compared to non-carriers. The CYP17A1 polymorphism rs11191548 demonstrated a significant association with LVMI in patients with arterial hypertension and preserved LVEF. Thus, CYP17A1 may contribute to cardiac hypertrophy in this clinical condition.
About the relation between implicit Theory of Mind & the comprehension of complement sentences (2010)
Poltrock, Silvana
Previous studies on the relation between language and social cognition have shown that children’s mastery of embedded sentential complements plays a causal role for the development of a Theory of Mind (ToM). Children start to succeed on complementation tasks in which they are required to report the content of an embedded clause in the second half of the fourth year. Traditional ToM tasks test the child’s ability to predict that a person who is holding a false belief (FB) about a situation will act "falsely". In these task, children do not represent FBs until the age of 4 years. According the linguistic determinism hypothesis, only the unique syntax of complement sentences provides the format for representing FBs. However, experiments measuring children’s looking behavior instead of their explicit predictions provided evidence that already 2-year olds possess an implicit ToM. This dissertation examined the question of whether there is an interrelation also between implicit ToM and the comprehension of complement sentences in typically developing German preschoolers. Two studies were conducted. In a correlational study (Study 1 ), 3-year-old children’s performance on a traditional (explicit) FB task, on an implicit FB task and on language tasks measuring children’s comprehension of tensed sentential complements were collected and tested for their interdependence. Eye-tracking methodology was used to assess implicit ToM by measuring participants’ spontaneous anticipatory eye movements while they were watching FB movies. Two central findings emerged. First, predictive looking (implicit ToM) was not correlated with complement mastery, although both measures were associated with explicit FB task performance. This pattern of results suggests that explicit, but not implicit ToM is language dependent. Second, as a group, 3-year-olds did not display implicit FB understanding. That is, previous findings on a precocious reasoning ability could not be replicated. This indicates that the characteristics of predictive looking tasks play a role for the elicitation of implicit FB understanding as the current task was completely nonverbal and as complex as traditional FB tasks. Study 2 took a methodological approach by investigating whether children display an earlier comprehension of sentential complements when using the same means of measurement as used in experimental tasks tapping implicit ToM, namely anticipatory looking. Two experiments were conducted. 3-year-olds were confronted either with a complement sentence expressing the protagonist’s FB (Exp. 1) or with a complex sentence expressing the protagonist’s belief without giving any information about the truth/ falsity of the belief (Exp. 2). Afterwards, their expectations about the protagonist’s future behavior were measured. Overall, implicit measures reveal no considerably earlier understanding of sentential complementation. Whereas 3-year-olds did not display a comprehension of complex sentences if these embedded a false proposition, children from 3;9 years on were proficient in processing complement sentences if the truth value of the embedded proposition could not be evaluated. This pattern of results suggests that (1) the linguistic expression of a person’s FB does not elicit implicit FB understanding and that (2) the assessment of the purely syntactic understanding of complement sentences is affected by competing reality information. In conclusion, this dissertation found no evidence that the implicit ToM is related to the comprehension of sentential complementation. The findings suggest that implicit ToM might be based on nonlinguistic processes. Results are discussed in the light of recently proposed dual-process models that assume two cognitive mechanisms that account for different levels of ToM task performance.
Accentual preferences and predictability: An acceptability study on split intransitivity in German (2015)
Verhoeven, Elisabeth ; Kügler, Frank
The difference in the default prosodic realization of simple sentences with unergative vs. unaccusative/passive verbs (assigning early nuclear accent with unaccusative/passive verbs but late nuclear accent with unergative verbs) is often related to the syntactic distinction of their nominative arguments as starting off in different hierarchical positions. Alternative accounts try to trace this prosodic variation back to asymmetries in the semantic or pragmatic contribution of the verb to an utterance. The present article investigates the interaction of the assignment of default nuclear accent with the predictability of the verb. In an experimental study testing the acceptability of nuclear accent assignment, we confirmed that the predictability of the verb influences accentual preferences (such that highly predictable verbs are preferably not accented). However, the experiment also reveals that the unaccusativity distinction cannot be accounted for by means of pragmatic phenomena of this type: the two verb classes are associated with distinct accentual patterns in the baseline condition, that is, without the predictability manipulation. (C) 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Acute Effects of Postactivation Potentiation on Strength and Speed Performance in Athletes (2013)
Lesinski, Melanie ; Muehlbauer, Thomas ; Buesch, Dirk ; Granacher, Urs
Background: The contractile history of a muscle or a muscle group can result in an acute enhancement of subsequent muscle force output. This phenomenon is referred to as postactivation potentiation (PAP) and it was frequently substantiated in original research manuscripts, systematic literature reviews, and meta-analyses. However, there is a lack in the literature regarding precise dose-response relations. This literature review describes the main determinants of PAP effects and additionally presents the state of the art regarding the acute effects of PAP protocols on measures of strength, power, and speed in subelite and elite athletes of different sport disciplines. Furthermore, an attempt is made to demonstrate evidence-based information concerning the design of effective PAP protocols. Methods: Our literature search included the electronic databases Pubmed, SportDiscus, and Google Scholar (1995 - March 2013). In total, 23 studies met the inclusionary criteria for review. Results: Findings from our literature review indicate that various conditioning activities produce acute PAP effects in subelite and particularly elite athletes. More specifically, conditioning activities that are characterised by multiple sets, moderate to high intensities (60 - 84 % of the one repetition maximum), and rest intervals of 7 - 10 min. following the conditioning activity have the potential to induce short-term improvements in muscle force output and sports performance. Conclusion: It is recommended that subelite and particularly elite athletes from strength, power, and speed disciplines apply specifically tailored conditioning activities during the acute preparation process for competition to induce performance enhancing PAP effects.
Acute Effects of Static Stretching on Muscle Strength and Power (2019)
Chaabene, Helmi ; Behm, David G. ; Negra, Yassine ; Granacher, Urs
The effects of static stretching (StS) on subsequent strength and power activities has been one of the most debated topics in sport science literature over the past decades. The aim of this review is (1) to summarize previous and current findings on the acute effects of StS on muscle strength and power performances; (2) to update readers’ knowledge related to previous caveats; and (3) to discuss the underlying physiological mechanisms of short-duration StS when performed as single-mode treatment or when integrated into a full warm-up routine. Over the last two decades, StS has been considered harmful to subsequent strength and power performances. Accordingly, it has been recommended not to apply StS before strength- and power-related activities. More recent evidence suggests that when performed as a single-mode treatment or when integrated within a full warm-up routine including aerobic activity, dynamic-stretching, and sport-specific activities, short-duration StS (≤60 s per muscle group) trivially impairs subsequent strength and power activities (∆1–2%). Yet, longer StS durations (>60 s per muscle group) appear to induce substantial and practically relevant declines in strength and power performances (∆4.0–7.5%). Moreover, recent evidence suggests that when included in a full warm-up routine, short-duration StS may even contribute to lower the risk of sustaining musculotendinous injuries especially with high-intensity activities (e.g., sprint running and change of direction speed). It seems that during short-duration StS, neuromuscular activation and musculotendinous stiffness appear not to be affected compared with long-duration StS. Among other factors, this could be due to an elevated muscle temperature induced by a dynamic warm-up program. More specifically, elevated muscle temperature leads to increased muscle fiber conduction-velocity and improved binding of contractile proteins (actin, myosin). Therefore, our previous understanding of harmful StS effects on subsequent strength and power activities has to be updated. In fact, short-duration StS should be included as an important warm-up component before the uptake of recreational sports activities due to its potential positive effect on flexibility and musculotendinous injury prevention. However, in high-performance athletes, short-duration StS has to be applied with caution due to its negligible but still prevalent negative effects on subsequent strength and power performances, which could have an impact on performance during competition.
Acute Effects of Static Stretching on Muscle Strength and Power (2019)
Chaabene, Helmi ; Behm, David G. ; Negra, Yassine ; Granacher, Urs
The effects of static stretching (StS) on subsequent strength and power activities has been one of the most debated topics in sport science literature over the past decades. The aim of this review is (1) to summarize previous and current findings on the acute effects of StS on muscle strength and power performances; (2) to update readers’ knowledge related to previous caveats; and (3) to discuss the underlying physiological mechanisms of short-duration StS when performed as single-mode treatment or when integrated into a full warm-up routine. Over the last two decades, StS has been considered harmful to subsequent strength and power performances. Accordingly, it has been recommended not to apply StS before strength- and power-related activities. More recent evidence suggests that when performed as a single-mode treatment or when integrated within a full warm-up routine including aerobic activity, dynamic-stretching, and sport-specific activities, short-duration StS (≤60 s per muscle group) trivially impairs subsequent strength and power activities (∆1–2%). Yet, longer StS durations (>60 s per muscle group) appear to induce substantial and practically relevant declines in strength and power performances (∆4.0–7.5%). Moreover, recent evidence suggests that when included in a full warm-up routine, short-duration StS may even contribute to lower the risk of sustaining musculotendinous injuries especially with high-intensity activities (e.g., sprint running and change of direction speed). It seems that during short-duration StS, neuromuscular activation and musculotendinous stiffness appear not to be affected compared with long-duration StS. Among other factors, this could be due to an elevated muscle temperature induced by a dynamic warm-up program. More specifically, elevated muscle temperature leads to increased muscle fiber conduction-velocity and improved binding of contractile proteins (actin, myosin). Therefore, our previous understanding of harmful StS effects on subsequent strength and power activities has to be updated. In fact, short-duration StS should be included as an important warm-up component before the uptake of recreational sports activities due to its potential positive effect on flexibility and musculotendinous injury prevention. However, in high-performance athletes, short-duration StS has to be applied with caution due to its negligible but still prevalent negative effects on subsequent strength and power performances, which could have an impact on performance during competition.
Addition goes where the big numbers are: evidence for a reversed operational momentum effect (2015)
Pinhas, Michal ; Shaki, Samuel ; Fischer, Martin H.
Number processing evokes spatial biases, both when dealing with single digits and in more complex mental calculations. Here we investigated whether these two biases have a common origin, by examining their flexibility. Participants pointed to the locations of arithmetic results on a visually presented line with an inverted, right-to-left number arrangement. We found directionally opposite spatial biases for mental arithmetic and for a parity task administered both before and after the arithmetic task. We discuss implications of this dissociation in our results for the task-dependent cognitive representation of numbers.
Affektive und kognitive Desensibilisierung als Konsequenz von Mediengewaltkonsum : eine experimentelle Untersuchung auf Basis lerntheoretischer Überlegungen (2014)
Busching, Robert
NutzerInnen von gewalthaltigen Medien geben einerseits oftmals zu, dass sie fiktionale, gewalthaltige Medien konsumieren, behaupten jedoch gleichzeitig, dass dies nicht ihr Verhalten außerhalb des Medienkontexts beeinflusst. Sie argumentieren, dass sie leicht zwischen Dingen, die im fiktionalen Kontext und Dingen, die in der Realität gelernt wurden, unterscheiden können. Im Kontrast zu diesen Aussagen zeigen Metanalysen Effektstärken im mittleren Bereich für den Zusammenhang zwischen Gewaltmedienkonsum und aggressivem Verhalten. Diese Ergebnisse können nur erklärt werden, wenn MediennutzerInnen gewalthaltige Lernerfahrungen auch außerhalb des Medienkontexts anwenden. Ein Prozess, der Lernerfahrungen innerhalb des Medienkontexts mit dem Verhalten in der realen Welt verknüpft, ist Desensibilisierung, die oftmals eine Reduktion des negativen Affektes gegenüber Gewalt definiert ist. Zur Untersuchung des Desensibilisierungsprozesses wurden vier Experimente durchgeführt. Die erste in dieser Arbeit untersuchte Hypothese war, dass je häufiger Personen Gewaltmedien konsumieren, desto weniger negativen Affekt zeigen sie gegenüber Bildern mit realer Gewalt. Jedoch wurde angenommen, dass diese Bewertung auf Darstellungen von realer Gewalt beschränkt ist und nicht bei Bildern ohne Gewaltbezug, die einen negativen Affekt auslösen, zu finden ist. Die zweite Hypothese bezog sich auf den Affekt während des Konsums von Mediengewalt. Hier wurde angenommen, dass besonders Personen, die Freude an Gewalt in den Medien empfinden weniger negativen Affekt gegenüber realen Gewaltdarstellungen zeigen. Die letzte Hypothese beschäftigte sich mit kognitiver Desensibilisierung und sagte vorher, dass Gewaltmedienkonsum zu einem Transfer von Reaktionen, die normalerweise gegenüber gewalthaltigen Reizen gezeigt werden, auf ursprünglich neutrale Reize führt. Das erste Experiment (N = 57) untersuchte, ob die habituelle Nutzung von gewalthaltigen Medien den selbstberichteten Affekt (Valenz und Aktivierung) gegenüber Darstellungen von realer Gewalt und nichtgewalthaltigen Darstellungen, die negativen Affekt auslösen, vorhersagt. Die habituelle Nutzung von gewalthaltigen Medien sagte weniger negative Valenz und weniger allgemeine Aktivierung gegenüber gewalthalten und nichtgewalthaltigen Bildern vorher. Das zweite Experiment (N = 103) untersuchte auch die Beziehung zwischen habituellem Gewaltmedienkonsum und den affektiven Reaktionen gegenüber Bildern realer Gewalt und negativen affektauslösenden Bildern. Als weiterer Prädiktor wurde der Affekt beim Betrachten von gewalthaltigen Medien hinzugefügt. Der Affekt gegenüber den Bildern wurde zusätzlich durch psychophysiologische Maße (Valenz: C: Supercilii; Aktivierung: Hautleitreaktion) erhoben. Wie zuvor sagte habitueller Gewaltmedienkonsum weniger selbstberichte Erregung und weniger negative Valenz für die gewalthaltigen und die negativen, gewalthaltfreien Bilder vorher. Die physiologischen Maßen replizierten dieses Ergebnis. Jedoch zeigte sich ein anderes Muster für den Affekt beim Konsum von Gewalt in den Medien. Personen, die Gewalt in den Medien stärker erfreut, zeigen eine Reduktion der Responsivität gegenüber Gewalt auf allen vier Maßen. Weiterhin war bei drei dieser vier Maße (selbstberichte Valenz, Aktivität des C. Supercilii und Hautleitreaktion) dieser Zusammenhang auf die gewalthaltigen Bilder beschränkt, mit keinem oder nur einem kleinen Effekt auf die negativen, aber nichtgewalthaltigen Bilder. Das dritte Experiment (N = 73) untersuchte den Affekt während die Teilnehmer ein Computerspiel spielten. Das Spiel wurde eigens für dieses Experiment programmiert, sodass einzelne Handlungen im Spiel mit der Aktivität des C. Supercilii, dem Indikator für negativen Affekt, in Bezug gesetzt werden konnten. Die Analyse des C. Supercilii zeigte, dass wiederholtes Durchführen von aggressiven Spielzügen zu einem Rückgang von negativen Affekt führte, der die aggressiven Spielhandlungen begleitete. Der negative Affekt während gewalthaltiger Spielzüge wiederum sagte die affektive Reaktion gegenüber Darstellungen von gewalthaltigen Bildern vorher, nicht jedoch gegenüber den negativen Bildern. Das vierte Experiment (N = 77) untersuchte kognitive Desensibilisierung, die die Entwicklung von Verknüpfungen zwischen neutralen und aggressiven Kognitionen beinhaltete. Die Teilnehmer spielten einen Ego-Shooter entweder auf einem Schiff- oder einem Stadtlevel. Die Beziehung zwischen den neutralen Konstrukten (Schiff/Stadt) und den aggressiven Kognitionen wurde mit einer lexikalischen Entscheidungsaufgabe gemessen. Das Spielen im Schiff-/Stadt-Level führte zu einer kürzen Reaktionszeit für aggressive Wörter, wenn sie einem Schiff- bzw. Stadtprime folgten. Dies zeigte, dass die im Spiel enthaltenen neutralen Konzepte mit aggressiven Knoten verknüpft werden. Die Ergebnisse dieser vier Experimente wurden diskutiert im Rahmen eines lerntheoretischen Ansatzes um Desensibilisierung zu konzeptualisieren.
Age and fitness level are strongest limitations of exercise capacity during inpatient cardiac rehabilitation (2014)
Völler, Heinz ; Salzwedel, Annett ; Reibis, Rona Katharina ; Kaminski, S. ; Buhlert, Hermann ; Eichler, Sarah ; Wegscheider, Karl
Age-Related Interference between the Selection of Input-Output Modality Mappings and Postural Control (2017)
Stelzel, Christine ; Schauenburg, Gesche ; Rapp, Michael Armin ; Heinzel, Stephan ; Granacher, Urs
Age-related decline in executive functions and postural control due to degenerative processes in the central nervous system have been related to increased fall-risk in old age. Many studies have shown cognitive-postural dual-task interference in old adults, but research on the role of specific executive functions in this context has just begun. In this study, we addressed the question whether postural control is impaired depending on the coordination of concurrent response-selection processes related to the compatibility of input and output modality mappings as compared to impairments related to working-memory load in the comparison of cognitive dual and single tasks. Specifically, we measured total center of pressure (CoP) displacements in healthy female participants aged 19–30 and 66–84 years while they performed different versions of a spatial one-back working memory task during semi-tandem stance on an unstable surface (i.e., balance pad) while standing on a force plate. The specific working-memory tasks comprised: (i) modality compatible single tasks (i.e., visual-manual or auditory-vocal tasks), (ii) modality compatible dual tasks (i.e., visual-manual and auditory-vocal tasks), (iii) modality incompatible single tasks (i.e., visual-vocal or auditory-manual tasks), and (iv) modality incompatible dual tasks (i.e., visual-vocal and auditory-manual tasks). In addition, participants performed the same tasks while sitting. As expected from previous research, old adults showed generally impaired performance under high working-memory load (i.e., dual vs. single one-back task). In addition, modality compatibility affected one-back performance in dual-task but not in single-task conditions with strikingly pronounced impairments in old adults. Notably, the modality incompatible dual task also resulted in a selective increase in total CoP displacements compared to the modality compatible dual task in the old but not in the young participants. These results suggest that in addition to effects of working-memory load, processes related to simultaneously overcoming special linkages between input- and output modalities interfere with postural control in old but not in young female adults. Our preliminary data provide further evidence for the involvement of cognitive control processes in postural tasks.
Age-Related Interference between the Selection of Input-Output Modality Mappings and Postural Control (2017)
Stelzel, Christine ; Schauenburg, Gesche ; Rapp, Michael Armin ; Heinzel, Stephan ; Granacher, Urs
Age-related decline in executive functions and postural control due to degenerative processes in the central nervous system have been related to increased fall-risk in old age. Many studies have shown cognitive-postural dual-task interference in old adults, but research on the role of specific executive functions in this context has just begun. In this study, we addressed the question whether postural control is impaired depending on the coordination of concurrent response-selection processes related to the compatibility of input and output modality mappings as compared to impairments related to working-memory load in the comparison of cognitive dual and single tasks. Specifically, we measured total center of pressure (CoP) displacements in healthy female participants aged 19–30 and 66–84 years while they performed different versions of a spatial one-back working memory task during semi-tandem stance on an unstable surface (i.e., balance pad) while standing on a force plate. The specific working-memory tasks comprised: (i) modality compatible single tasks (i.e., visual-manual or auditory-vocal tasks), (ii) modality compatible dual tasks (i.e., visual-manual and auditory-vocal tasks), (iii) modality incompatible single tasks (i.e., visual-vocal or auditory-manual tasks), and (iv) modality incompatible dual tasks (i.e., visual-vocal and auditory-manual tasks). In addition, participants performed the same tasks while sitting. As expected from previous research, old adults showed generally impaired performance under high working-memory load (i.e., dual vs. single one-back task). In addition, modality compatibility affected one-back performance in dual-task but not in single-task conditions with strikingly pronounced impairments in old adults. Notably, the modality incompatible dual task also resulted in a selective increase in total CoP displacements compared to the modality compatible dual task in the old but not in the young participants. These results suggest that in addition to effects of working-memory load, processes related to simultaneously overcoming special linkages between input- and output modalities interfere with postural control in old but not in young female adults. Our preliminary data provide further evidence for the involvement of cognitive control processes in postural tasks.
Alcohol in the Aging Brain (2019)
Mende, Melinda Alicia
As our society grows older new challenges for medicine and healthcare emerge. Agerelated changes of the body have been observed in essential body functions, particularly in the loco-motor system, in the cardiovascular system and in cognitive functions concerning both brain plasticity and changes in behavior. Nutrition and lifestyle, such as nicotine intake and chronic alcohol consumption, also contribute to biological changes in the brain. This review addresses the effect of alcohol consumption on cognitive decline, changes in brain plasticity in the aging brain and on cardiovascular health in aging. Thus, studies on the interplay of chronic alcohol intake and either cognitive decline or cognitive preservation are outlined. Because of the inconsistency in the literature of whether alcohol consumption preserves cognitive functions in the aging brain or whether it accelerates cognitive decline, it is crucial to consider individual contributing factors such as culture, health and lifestyle in future studies.
Alcohol in the Aging Brain (2019)
Mende, Melinda Alicia
As our society grows older new challenges for medicine and healthcare emerge. Agerelated changes of the body have been observed in essential body functions, particularly in the loco-motor system, in the cardiovascular system and in cognitive functions concerning both brain plasticity and changes in behavior. Nutrition and lifestyle, such as nicotine intake and chronic alcohol consumption, also contribute to biological changes in the brain. This review addresses the effect of alcohol consumption on cognitive decline, changes in brain plasticity in the aging brain and on cardiovascular health in aging. Thus, studies on the interplay of chronic alcohol intake and either cognitive decline or cognitive preservation are outlined. Because of the inconsistency in the literature of whether alcohol consumption preserves cognitive functions in the aging brain or whether it accelerates cognitive decline, it is crucial to consider individual contributing factors such as culture, health and lifestyle in future studies.
  • 1 to 20

OPUS4 Logo  KOBV Logo  OAI Logo  DINI Zertifikat 2007  OA Netzwerk Logo

    • Institutional Repository
    • University Bibliography
    • University Library
    • Policy
    • Contact
    • Imprint
    • Privacy Policy
    • Accessibility

    Login