TY - JOUR A1 - Vöhringer, Matthias A1 - Schütz, Astrid A1 - Geßler, Sarah A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - SREIS-D BT - die deutschsprachige Version der Self-Rated Emotional Intelligence Scale BT - German version of the Self-Rated Emotional Intelligence Scale (SREIS) in a clinical sample and a control group JF - Diagnostica N2 - Emotionale Intelligenz (EI) ist ein zentraler Prädiktor psychischer Gesundheit. Im deutschsprachigen Raum lag bislang keine am Vier-Facetten-Modell der EI orientierte Selbstbeschreibungsskala vor, die an klinischen und nicht-klinischen Gruppen getestet wurde. Die Self-Rated Emotional Intelligence Scale (SREIS) ist mit 19 Items ein ökonomisch einsetzbares Instrument. Die Skala wurde ins Deutsche übertragen und psychometrisch überprüft. Außerdem wurde die SREIS erstmals an einer klinischen Population getestet. Auch werden erstmals differenzierte Ergebnisse zu den vier EI-Facetten vorgelegt. Die Ergebnisse bestätigen die Faktorenstruktur der englischen Originalskala. Die Reliabilität der Gesamtskala ist als gut einzustufen. Validität wird durch erwartungskonforme Korrelationen mit anderen EI-Maßen sowie klinischen Parametern belegt. Durch Diskriminationsfähigkeit zwischen klinischer Stichprobe und nicht-klinischer Kontrollgruppe zeigt die Skala zusätzlich klinische Relevanz. Skalare Messinvarianz zwischen beiden Gruppen liegt vor. Die SREIS-D ist ein ökonomisch einsetzbares Selbstberichtsmaß zur Erfassung von Facetten der EI im klinischen und subklinischen Bereich. N2 - Emotional intelligence (EI) is significantly linked to mental health. However, many existing EI measures are not based on integrative theoretical approaches and they lack clinical testing. The present study presents the German version of a 19-item self-report scale based on the widely accepted four-branch model of EI. It is the first study to use the scale in a large clinical sample (n = 338) in addition to a nonclinical sample (n = 218) and to show specific relations with the four facets of EI. Factorial structure and psychometric criteria were evaluated in both samples. The results confirm the factor structure of the original scale. The reliability of the total scale measured with McDonald’s omega is good. Validity is shown based on correlations with other EI measures and clinical indicators. Furthermore, the SREIS-D discriminates between clinical and nonclinical groups. Scalar measurement invariance was found between both groups. The scale can be considered useful and economic for use in clinical emotions and nonclinical assessment of overall EI and of the four facets of perceiving emotions, using emotions to facilitate thinking, understanding emotion and managing emotions. KW - Emotionale Intelligenz KW - Emotionsregulation KW - Emotionswahrnehmung KW - Skala KW - psychische Gesundheit KW - emotional intelligence KW - emotion regulation KW - emotion-perception KW - self-report KW - mental health Y1 - 2020 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000248 SN - 0012-1924 SN - 2190-622X VL - 66 IS - 3 SP - 200 EP - 210 PB - Hogrefe CY - Göttingen ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela A1 - Rentzsch, Katrin A1 - Asendorpf, Jens B. A1 - Penke, Lars T1 - Good Enough for an Affair. Self-Enhancement of Attractiveness, Interest in Potential Mates and Popularity as a Mate JF - European journal of personality N2 - Using data from the Berlin Speed Dating Study, we tested rival hypotheses concerning the effects of self-enhancement of attractiveness on dating outcomes. Three hundred eighty-two participants took part in one of the 17 speed-dating sessions. After each speed-dating interaction, participants indicated how interesting they found the respective person as a long-term and short-term partner. Using social relations analyses, we computed perceiver effects (being more or less choosy) and target effects (being rated as more or less interesting) of long-term and short-term partner ratings. Self-enhancement was operationalized as the discrepancy between self-rated attractiveness and four components of actual attractiveness (observer-rated facial and vocal attractiveness, height and body mass index). Results indicated that self-enhancers were less choosy with respect to their interest for short-term partners, which was especially true for men, but more choosy with respect to long-term partners. With regard to popularity as a mate, potential partners indicated that they found self-enhancers more interesting as short-term partners but not as long-term partners. As self-enhancement is a key component of narcissism, these results are consistent with findings that narcissists perceive many sexual affairs as an achievement, while preferring selected ‘trophy’ long-term partners, and narcissists have a charming appeal for short-term, but not lasting, social relationships. KW - self-enhancement KW - physical attractiveness KW - mating KW - speed dating KW - social relations analyses Y1 - 2016 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2029 SN - 0890-2070 SN - 1099-0984 VL - 30 SP - 12 EP - 18 PB - Wiley-Blackwell CY - Hoboken ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi T1 - Shades of narcissistic dishonesty BT - Grandiose versus vulnerable narcissism and the role of self-conscious emotions JF - Journal of economic psychology : research in economic psychology and behavioral economics N2 - Honesty is crucial to navigate the social realm, yet certain individuals - those high in narcissism - tend to engage in dishonesty. In two studies (total N = 910), we aimed to zoom in on the link between grandiose versus vulnerable narcissism and dishonesty, further clarifying the role of the key self-conscious emotions shame and guilt in mediating this link. Using behavioral indicators of dishonesty, namely, actual cheating in a math task (Study 1) and a coin-tossing task (Study 2), we consistently found that the relationship between grandiose narcissism and cheating was positively mediated by guilt, indicating that grandiose narcissists engaged in more dishonest behavior due to a lack of guilt. Furthermore, the relationship between vulnerable narcissism and cheating was negatively mediated by shame, but only when task success depended on performance (Study 1) rather than luck (Study 2). Results underscore the importance of differentiating between distinct facets of narcissism and highlight the role of self-conscious emotions in the narcissism-dishonesty link. KW - Narcissism KW - Dishonesty KW - Shame KW - Guilt KW - Unethical behavior Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2018.06.003 SN - 0167-4870 SN - 1872-7719 VL - 71 SP - 148 EP - 158 PB - Elsevier CY - Amsterdam ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rudolph, Almut A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela A1 - Schütz, Astrid T1 - I like myself, I really do (at least right now) BT - development and validation of a brief and revised (German-language) version of the State Self-Esteem Scale JF - European journal of psychological assessment : EJPA N2 - In five studies, we evaluated the psychometric properties of a revised German version of the State Self-Esteem Scale (SSES; Heatherton & Polivy, 1991). In Study 1, the results of a confirmatory factor analysis on the original scale revealed poor model fit and poor construct validity in a student sample that resembled those in the literature; thus, a revised 15-item version was developed (i.e., the SSES-R) and thoroughly validated. Study 2 showed a valid three-factor structure (Performance, Social, and Appearance) and good internal consistency of the SSES-R. Correlations between subscales of trait and state SE empirically supported the scale's construct validity. Temporal stability and intrapersonal sensitivity of the scale to naturally occurring events were investigated in Study 3. Intrapersonat sensitivity of the scale to experimentally induced changes in state SE was uncovered in Study 4 via social feedback (acceptance vs. rejection) and performance feedback (positive vs. negative). In Study 5, the scale's interpersonal sensitivity was confirmed by comparing depressed and healthy individuals. Finally, the usefulness of the SSES-R was demonstrated by assessing SE instability as calculated from repeated measures of state SE. KW - self-esteem KW - state self-esteem KW - State Self-Esteem Scale KW - positive affect KW - negative affect Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000501 SN - 1015-5759 SN - 2151-2426 VL - 36 IS - 1 SP - 196 EP - 206 PB - Hogrefe CY - Göttingen ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rentzsch, Katrin A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - Top down or bottom up? BT - evidence from the longitudinal development of global and domain-specific self-esteem in adulthood JF - Journal of personality and social psychology / American Psychological Association N2 - Classical theoretical perspectives have implied that either global self-esteem has an impact on domain-specific self-esteem (top-down) or domain-specific self-esteem affects global self-esteem (bottom-up). The goal of the present research was to investigate whether classical top-down and bottom-up approaches could withstand a thorough test. To do so, we applied elaborate analytical methods in a four-wave longitudinal study across 6 years with preregistered hypotheses and data analyses. We analyzed data from N = 1,417 German participants (30.6% men, median of 12 to 13 years of education) with an average age of 47.0 years (SD = 12.4, range 18 to 88) at intake. Analyses using latent variable approaches for modeling intraindividual change provided evidence of top-down effects only. For example, participants with higher global self-esteem exhibited an increase in performance self-esteem but not vice versa. Our results also provided evidence of "vertical" associations between global and domain-specific self-esteem, that is, parallel development within the same time frame. In addition, the analyses revealed high rank order stability and a substantial trait component in global self-esteem and the self-esteem domains. The present findings have important theoretical and practical implications for the stability and development of self-esteem in adulthood and advance the understanding of global and domain-specific self-esteem in personality theory. KW - self-esteem KW - domain-specific KW - top-down and bottom-up KW - stability KW - adulthood Y1 - 2022 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000393 SN - 0022-3514 SN - 1939-1315 VL - 122 IS - 4 SP - 714 EP - 730 PB - American Psychological Association CY - Washington ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rentzsch, Katrin A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - Stability and change in domain-specific Self-esteem and global self-esteem JF - European journal of personality N2 - A notable uptick of interest in the stability of self-esteem has been observed over the past few years. Most researchers, however, have focused on unidimensional rather than multidimensional conceptualizations of self-esteem. The paucity of empirical research is surprising given conflicting theoretical perspectives on the stability of self-esteem. The goal of the present study was to thoroughly disentangle different conceptualizations of self-esteem and test opposing classical theories on (i) the stability and (ii) the direction of mutual influence of these different forms of self-esteem. We analysed two-year longitudinal data from participants (N=644 at T1, N=241 at T2) with an average age of 47.0years (SD=12.4). Analyses using a latent variable approach revealed that the domains of self-esteem were relatively stable in terms of rank order and mean levels. In fact, the size of the stability coefficients was comparable to that of other trait measures that have been reported in the literature and paralleled the stability observed for global self-esteem. Results did not provide support for either top-down or bottom-up effects between domain-specific and global self-esteem. These findings have important theoretical and practical implications regarding the stability and development of self-esteem in adulthood and advance the understanding of self-esteem in personality theory. (c) 2018 European Association of Personality Psychology KW - self-esteem KW - stability KW - multidimensional KW - domain specific KW - adulthood Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2167 SN - 0890-2070 SN - 1099-0984 VL - 32 IS - 4 SP - 353 EP - 370 PB - Wiley CY - Hoboken ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Mota, Simon A1 - Leckelt, Marius A1 - Geukes, Katharina A1 - Nestler, Steffen A1 - Humberg, Sarah A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela A1 - Schmukle, Stefan C. A1 - Back, Mitja D. T1 - A comprehensive examination of narcissists’ self-perceived and actual socioemotional cognition ability JF - Collabra: Psychology N2 - Narcissists are assumed to lack the motivation and ability to share and understand the mental states of others. Prior empirical research, however, has yielded inconclusive findings and has differed with respect to the specific aspects of narcissism and socioemotional cognition that have been examined. Here, we propose a differentiated facet approach that can be applied across research traditions and that distinguishes between facets of narcissism (agentic vs. antagonistic) on the one hand, and facets of socioemotional cognition ability (SECA; self-perceived vs. actual) on the other. Using five nonclinical samples in two studies (total N = 602), we investigated the effect of facets of grandiose narcissism on aspects of socioemotional cognition across measures of affective and cognitive empathy, Theory of Mind, and emotional intelligence, while also controlling for general reasoning ability. Across both studies, agentic facets of narcissism were found to be positively related to perceived SECA, whereas antagonistic facets of narcissism were found to be negatively related to perceived SECA. However, both narcissism facets were negatively related to actual SECA. Exploratory condition-based regression analyses further showed that agentic narcissists had a higher directed discrepancy between perceived and actual SECA: They self-enhanced their socio-emotional capacities. Implications of these results for the multifaceted theoretical understanding of the narcissism-SECA link are discussed. KW - narcissism KW - socioemotional cognition KW - self-perceived ability KW - actual ability KW - empathy KW - emotional intelligence KW - self-enhancement Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.174 SN - 2474-7394 VL - 5 IS - 1 PB - University of California Press CY - Oakland ER - TY - GEN A1 - Morf, Carolyn C. A1 - Schürch, Eva A1 - Küfner, Albrecht A1 - Siegrist, Philip A1 - Vater, Aline A1 - Back, Mitja A1 - Mestel, Robert A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - Expanding the nomological net of the pathological narcissism inventory BT - German validation and extension in a clinical inpatient sample T2 - Postprints der Universität Potsdam : Humanwissenschaftliche Reihe N2 - The Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI) is a multidimensional measure for assessing grandiose and vulnerable features in narcissistic pathology. The aim of the present research was to construct and validate a German translation of the PNI and to provide further information on the PNI's nomological net. Findings from a first study confirm the psychometric soundness of the PNI and replicate its seven-factor first-order structure. A second-order structure was also supported but with several equivalent models. A second study investigating associations with a broad range of measures (DSM Axis I and II constructs, emotions, personality traits, interpersonal and dysfunctional behaviors, and well-being) supported the concurrent validity of the PNI. Discriminant validity with the Narcissistic Personality Inventory was also shown. Finally, in a third study an extension in a clinical inpatient sample provided further evidence that the PNI is a useful tool to assess the more pathological end of narcissism. T3 - Zweitveröffentlichungen der Universität Potsdam : Humanwissenschaftliche Reihe - 410 KW - narcissism KW - assessment KW - Pathological Narcissism Inventory KW - construct validity KW - nomological network Y1 - 2018 U6 - http://nbn-resolving.de/urn/resolver.pl?urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus4-405182 IS - 410 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Morf, Carolyn C. A1 - Schurch, Eva A1 - Kufener, Albrecht A1 - Siegrist, Philip A1 - Vater, Aline A1 - Back, Mitja A1 - Mestel, Robert A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - Expanding the Nomological Net of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory: German Validation and Extension in a Clinical Inpatient Sample JF - Assessment KW - narcissism KW - assessment KW - Pathological Narcissism Inventory KW - construct validity KW - nomological network Y1 - 2017 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191115627010 SN - 1073-1911 SN - 1552-3489 VL - 24 SP - 419 EP - 443 PB - Sage Publ. CY - Thousand Oaks ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Malesza, Marta A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - The convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of the Discounting Inventory compared to the traditional discounting measures JF - Journal of public health N2 - Aim The Discounting Inventory was developed to provide researchers and practitioners with a standardized tool to asses individual differences in delay, probabilistic, effort and social discounting, all related to behavioral impulsivity. Convergent and discriminant validity of the Discounting Inventory was evaluated by comparing its associations with external variables to those obtained using standardized discounting measures with the same external variables. Subjects and methods A sample of 347 volunteers was examined. The first questionnaire completed by all participants was the Discounting Inventory. Individuals also completed the traditional discounting measure using pairs of hypothetical choices. The external measures included the Sensation Seeking Scale, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, Eysenck Impulsivity Venturesomeness Empathy Questionnaire, Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised, and Temperament and Character Inventory. Results The results revealed that although almost all correlations were significant for both the Discounting Inventory and the traditional discounting measures, most of the correlations between external variables and the Discounting Inventory were significantly higher than those between external variables and traditional discounting measures. However, both discounting measures were most strongly correlated with external measures of impulsivity, which is not surprising given the fact that discounting is seen as a behavioral impulsivity. Finally, most relationships between the Discounting Inventory and external variables were incrementally valid over the traditional discounting measures. Conclusion The Discounting Inventory can help to diagnose problems with behavioral impulsivity. KW - Discounting inventory KW - Convergent validity KW - Discriminant validity KW - Incremental validity KW - Discounting Y1 - 2020 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1007/s10389-020-01306-y SN - 2198-1833 SN - 1613-2238 VL - 30 IS - 2 SP - 423 EP - 433 PB - Springer CY - Berlin ; Heidelberg ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kratky, Nicole A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - How are parental functioning and single parenthood associated with court outcomes? BT - an analysis of child protection cases JF - Child Abuse & Neglect N2 - Impaired parental functioning and single parenthood are considered risk factors for child maltreatment and being involved in the child protection context. Past research has shown that an impaired mental functioning and being a single parent are indicators of limited parenting resources. These risk factors are likely to be considered by family judges, which might lead to more intrusive court decisions concerning parental custody. To date, court data have rarely been investigated. The present study examined parental mental health and single parenthood using data from family law proceedings. The role of the fathers has been understudied and the few existing studies yielded contradictory results with respect to fathers’ involvement as risk or protective factor. Therefore, the study included both fathers’ data and mothers’ data. A total of 220 child protection court files with 343 affected children were coded using a category system. Parental mental health was coded as parental functioning in daily life and was significantly associated with the court outcome. Multilevel mediation analyses showed a significant indirect effect of maternal functioning on the intrusiveness of the court decisions via child maltreatment. Single motherhood moderated the effect: The indirect effect was more pronounced for single mothers. This study contributes to a better understanding of the population getting before court and the judicial process. Psychological attributes do play a role in the decision-making of judges; and taking the role of the fathers into account is necessary. KW - Child maltreatment KW - Child protection KW - Court KW - Parental mental health KW - Single parenthood Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.07.015 SN - 0145-2134 SN - 1873-7757 VL - 84 SP - 95 EP - 105 PB - Elsevier CY - Oxford ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kratky, Nicole A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - A court file analysis of child protection cases BT - what do children say? JF - Child & family social work N2 - Children's participation in legal proceedings affecting them personally has been gaining importance. So far, a primary research concern has been how children experience their participation in court proceedings. However, little is known about the child's voice itself: Are children able to clearly express their wishes, and if so, what do they say in child protection cases? In this study, we extracted information about children's statements from court file data of 220 child protection cases in Germany. We found 182 children were asked about their wishes. The majority of the statements found came either from reports of the guardians ad litem or from judicial records of the child hearings. Using content analysis, three main aspects of the statements were extracted: wishes concerning main place of residence, wishes about whom to have or not contact with, and children granting decision-making authority to someone else. Children's main focus was on their parents, but others (e.g., relatives and foster care providers) were also mentioned. Intercoder agreement was substantial. Making sure that child hearings are as informative as possible is in the child's best interest. Therefore, the categories developed herein might help professionals to ask questions more precisely relevant to the child. KW - children's participation KW - child protection KW - child's voice KW - child KW - welfare KW - court files KW - family court Y1 - 2020 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.12744 SN - 1356-7500 SN - 1365-2206 VL - 25 IS - S1 SP - 169 EP - 177 PB - Wiley-Blackwell CY - Oxford ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Jung, Janis Moritz A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - Prosocial behavior as a protective factor against peers' acceptance of aggression in the development of aggressive behavior in childhood and adolescence JF - Journal of adolescence KW - Resilience KW - Protective factor KW - Childhood KW - Adolescence KW - Longitudinal KW - Germany Y1 - 2019 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.06.002 SN - 0140-1971 SN - 1095-9254 VL - 74 SP - 146 EP - 153 PB - Elsevier CY - London ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Heinze, Peter Eric A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - Validation of an implicit measure of antagonistic narcissism JF - Journal of research in personality N2 - Narcissism has traditionally been assessed using explicit measures, yet contemporary measures are limited in their ability to capture people's automatic (i.e., implicit) self-evaluations. Here, we propose the antagonistic narcissism Implicit Association Test (AN-IAT). Three studies (N = 1082) using self-, informant-reports, and other implicit measures tested the psychometric properties of the AN-IAT. The AN-IAT showed high internal consistency and good temporal stability. The measure was positively associated with (antagonistic) narcissism, aggression, and lack of empathy, but unrelated to communal, pathological, and agentic narcissism as well as self-esteem. The AN-IAT predicted self- and informantratings of aggression and empathy beyond self-reports of antagonistic and agentic narcissism, and agreeableness. Together, the antagonistic narcissism IAT is a promising addition to the assessment of narcissism. KW - narcissism KW - antagonistic KW - implicit self-concept of personality KW - IAT Y1 - 2020 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103993 SN - 0092-6566 SN - 1095-7251 VL - 88 PB - Elsevier CY - San Diego ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Zeigler-Hill, Virgil A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - I’m merciful, am I not? BT - Facets of narcissism and forgiveness revisited JF - Journal of research in personality N2 - Narcissists are said to be particularly unforgiving, yet previous research remains inconclusive. This is likely because most previous studies focused on narcissism as a unitary construct, thereby neglecting its multiple facets. The present study (N = 1101) thus aimed to clarify the nuanced associations between different facets of narcissism and forgiveness, the latter being assessed via self-report and non-self-report measures. The results of a structural equation model (SEM) showed that antagonistic aspects of narcissism were negatively correlated with explicit forgiveness. Importantly, agentic as well as communal aspects of narcissism were positively correlated with explicit forgiveness. Aspects of narcissistic personality were not correlated with implicit forgiveness. Results suggest that not all facets of narcissism are associated with an unforgiving stance. KW - Narcissism KW - Forgiveness KW - Social relationships KW - Narcissistic entitlement KW - Conflict Y1 - 2017 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.07.007 SN - 0092-6566 SN - 1095-7251 VL - 70 SP - 166 EP - 173 PB - Elsevier CY - San Diego ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - A wolf in sheep’s clothing? BT - Communal narcissism and positive implicit self-views in the communal domain JF - Journal of research in personality N2 - Communal narcissists possess the unique belief in their capability to bring about freedom to the world, and so see themselves as “saints”. To examine if this communal self-view extends to the more automatic component of self-evaluation, that is, a person’s implicit self-view, the present study (N = 701) tested the extent to which communal narcissism was associated with explicit communal self-ratings and implicit associations between the self and communal attributes. The latent correlation between communal narcissism and explicit communal self-views was strongly positive, yet no such relationship emerged for implicit communal self-views. These findings support the notion that communal narcissism may represent an effort to gain favorable appraisals from others in the absence of a genuine communal self-view. KW - Narcissism KW - Communion KW - Agency KW - Self KW - Implicit Association Test Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.07.004 SN - 0092-6566 SN - 1095-7251 VL - 76 SP - 17 EP - 21 PB - Elsevier CY - San Diego ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - I can see clearly now BT - clarity of transgression-related motivations JF - Personality and individual differences : an international journal of research into the structure and development of personality, and the causation of individual differences N2 - Past research has revealed that narcissists lack forgiveness. However, little is known about factors that might either buffer or, more critically, enhance the link between narcissism and lack of forgiveness. To address this gap in the literature, the present studies focused on the moderating role of clarity of transgression-related motivations. In an original and a replication study (Ns = 509 and 532, respectively), participants rated their levels of narcissistic admiration and rivalry and recalled a personal episode in which someone had hurt them. Subsequently, participants reported on their lack of forgiveness toward their transgressor. Response speed to these ratings served as an indirect clarity measure, with faster responses indicating greater clarity. In both studies, narcissistic rivalry (but not admiration) was positively related to lack of forgiveness and this relationship was stronger among individuals who were clear about their transgression-related motivations. Results inform our understanding of socio-emotional factors that contribute to narcissists' lack of forgiveness following interpersonal hurt. KW - Narcissism KW - Forgiveness KW - Interpersonal transgressions KW - Social relationships KW - Indirect measurement Y1 - 2016 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.10.010 SN - 0191-8869 VL - 105 SP - 280 EP - 286 PB - Elsevier CY - Oxford ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Schröder-Abe, Michela T1 - Agentic to the core? BT - facets of narcissism and positive implicit self-views in the agentic domain JF - Journal of research in personality N2 - Researchers are still divided over whether narcissists possess positive or negative implicit self-views. Seemingly resolving this issue, Campbell et al. (2007) have demonstrated that narcissism is in fact related to higher implicit self-esteem as long as the implicit measure reflects agency. The present study used a large (N = 730) sample, carefully controlled stimuli, improved statistical analyses, and examined narcissism at the facet-level, but results did not replicate those of Campbell et al. In fact, the latent correlation between narcissism and implicit agency was close to zero, whereas the positive correlation between narcissism and explicit agency was replicated. We conclude that narcissists’ implicit self-views may be more neutral than positive or may depend on other contextual factors. KW - Narcissism KW - Agency KW - Communion KW - Implicit self-esteem KW - IAT Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.02.006 SN - 0092-6566 SN - 1095-7251 VL - 74 SP - 78 EP - 82 PB - Elsevier CY - San Diego ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Fatfouta, Ramzi A1 - Rentzsch, Katrin A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - Narcissus oeconomicus: BT - facets of narcissism and socio-economic decision-making JF - Journal of research in personality N2 - How do narcissists respond to monetary rewards and are there differences in fairness sensitivity between different facets of narcissism? The present study (N = 287) investigated these questions using the Ultimatum Game, a behavioral decision-making task involving the presentation of advantageous and disadvantageous financial offers. The results of multilevel modeling revealed that individual differences in narcissism modulated responders’ game decisions: Individuals high in narcissism, particularly narcissistic rivalry, were more likely to accept monetary offers and this effect was even more pronounced for comparatively unfair offers. Results extend previous findings, suggesting that narcissists are hypersensitive to rewards and pay close attention how to maximize their personal profit rather than to enforce fairness norms. KW - Narcissism KW - Fairness KW - Ultimatum Game KW - Decision making KW - Social cognition Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.05.002 SN - 0092-6566 SN - 1095-7251 VL - 75 SP - 12 EP - 16 PB - Elsevier CY - San Diego ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Ewert, Christina A1 - Vater, Annika A1 - Schröder-Abé, Michela T1 - Self-compassion and coping BT - a meta-analysis JF - Mindfulness N2 - Objectives: Self-compassion, a positive and caring attitude toward oneself, has been identified as an important correlate of coping in stressful situations. High self-compassion is related to higher use of adaptive and less maladaptive coping in demanding or painful situations. However, estimates of these relations in terms of specific adaptive and maladaptive coping strategies have remained inconclusive. Therefore, the present meta-analysis investigates the relation between self-compassion and different forms of adaptive and maladaptive coping. It also takes into account potential moderators such as age, gender, and regional background. Methods: A systematic literature search resulted in k = 136 samples with an overall sample size of N = 38,913. Random-effects models were used to integrate the z-transformed Pearson correlation coefficients. Results: Analyses yielded a positive correlation between self-compassion and adaptive coping (r = .306) and a negative correlation between self-compassion and maladaptive coping (r = - .500). The association of self-compassion with emotional approach coping was positive (r = .340), as was the association with problem-focused coping (r = .205). Participants' age appeared to be a significant moderator of the relation between self-compassion and coping. Conclusions: Self-compassion is important for understanding the mechanisms involved in coping with stress and demanding life events. The size and direction of correlations depend on the coping strategies considered, with protective effects of self-compassion with respect to maladaptive coping being the most pronounced. Further research should examine the relation between self-compassion and coping in more detail and focus on additional moderators. KW - Meta-analysis KW - Self-compassion KW - Coping KW - Stress regulation Y1 - 2021 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-020-01563-8 SN - 1868-8527 SN - 1868-8535 VL - 12 IS - 5 SP - 1063 EP - 1077 PB - Springer CY - Dordrecht ER -