TY - JOUR A1 - Abramova, Olga A1 - Wagner, Amina A1 - Olt, Christian M. A1 - Buxmann, Peter T1 - One for all, all for one BT - social considerations in user acceptance of contact tracing apps using longitudinal evidence from Germany and Switzerland JF - International Journal of Information Management N2 - We propose a conceptual model of acceptance of contact tracing apps based on the privacy calculus perspective. Moving beyond the duality of personal benefits and privacy risks, we theorize that users hold social considerations (i.e., social benefits and risks) that underlie their acceptance decisions. To test our propositions, we chose the context of COVID-19 contact tracing apps and conducted a qualitative pre-study and longitudinal quantitative main study with 589 participants from Germany and Switzerland. Our findings confirm the prominence of individual privacy calculus in explaining intention to use and actual behavior. While privacy risks are a significant determinant of intention to use, social risks (operationalized as fear of mass surveillance) have a notably stronger impact. Our mediation analysis suggests that social risks represent the underlying mechanism behind the observed negative link between individual privacy risks and contact tracing apps' acceptance. Furthermore, we find a substantial intention–behavior gap. KW - digital contact tracing KW - privacy calculus KW - longitudinal study KW - privacy risks KW - surveillance KW - intention-behavior gap Y1 - 2022 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2022.102473 SN - 0268-4012 SN - 1873-4707 VL - 64 SP - 1 EP - 16 PB - Elsevier CY - Kidlington ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Schulz, Anika D. A1 - Schöllgen, Ina A1 - Wendsche, Johannes A1 - Fay, Doris A1 - Wegge, Jürgen T1 - The dynamics of social stressors and detachment BT - long term mechanisms impacting well-being JF - International journal of stress management N2 - This study examines the long-term dynamics of social stressors at work, psychological detachment, and their impact on employee well-being. Previous research has shown that social stressors are detrimental for employee well-being and the ability to mentally detach from work. However, longitudinal studies in this field are scarce, and typically, they only explore whether the level of stressors, or of detachment, at a given point in time has an effect on outcomes. That stressors and detachment may change over time, and that this change may have an independent effect in the process, has rarely been taken into consideration. Thus, it is unclear to what extent long-term dynamic effects also play a role in these relations. To address this question, we investigated whether change in detachment explains the long-term indirect relationship of change in perceived social stressors with change in emotional exhaustion and mental well-being. Data were taken from a longitudinal study of N = 246 registered nurses with up to 3 measurements over 1 year. Analyses were conducted with latent difference scores using a proportional change model. Results revealed that a decline in psychological detachment mediated the long-term effects of increases in social stressors at the workplace on subsequent change in emotional exhaustion and mental well-being. Thus, our study provides initial evidence for the underlying long-term dynamic nature of relationships among social stressors, detachment, and employee well-being, highlighting the incremental explanatory power of change in social stressors and in detachment, above and beyond their respective levels, in predicting change in well-being. KW - psychological detachment KW - job stress KW - emotional exhaustion KW - well-being KW - longitudinal study Y1 - 2021 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1037/str0000216 SN - 1072-5245 SN - 1573-3424 VL - 28 IS - 3 SP - 207 EP - 219 PB - American Psychological Association CY - Washington ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Krahé, Barbara A1 - Berger, Anja T1 - Longitudinal pathways of sexual victimization, sexual self-esteem, and depression in women and men JF - Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy N2 - Objective: This article presents a longitudinal analysis of the links between sexual assault victimization, depression, and sexual self-esteem by examining their cross-lagged paths among both men and women. Method: Male and female college students (N = 2,425) in Germany participated in the study that comprised 3 data waves in their first, second, and third year of university, separated by 12-month intervals. Sexual assault victimization was assessed at Time 1 (T1) since the age of 14 and at Time 2 (T2) and Time 3 (T3) for the last 12 months. Depression and sexual self-esteem were measured at each wave. Results: Random-intercept cross-lagged panel analyses, controlling for individual differences in depression and sexual self-esteem, showed that sexual assault at T1 predicted depression and lower sexual self-esteem at T2, and depression and lower self-esteem at T2 predicted sexual assault victimization at T3. In addition, significant paths were found from T1 depression to T2 sexual assault victimization and from T2 sexual assault victimization to depression at T3. Sexual victimization at T1 was indirectly linked to sexual victimization at T3 via depression at T2. Both depression and sexual self-esteem at T1 were indirectly linked to sexual victimization at T3. The paths did not differ significantly between men and women. Conclusion: Sexual assault victimization was shown to be a risk factor for both depression as a general mental health indicator and lowered sexual self-esteem as a specific outcome in the domain of sexuality. Moreover, depression and sexual self-esteem increased the vulnerability for sexual assault victimization, which has implications for prevention and intervention efforts. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. KW - sexual assault victimization KW - depression KW - sexual self-esteem KW - longitudinal study KW - Germany Y1 - 2016 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000198 SN - 1942-9681 SN - 1942-969X VL - 9 IS - 2 SP - 147 EP - 155 PB - American Psychological Association CY - Washington ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Holl, Anna Katharina A1 - Kirsch, Fabian A1 - Rohlf, Helena L. A1 - Krahe, Barbara A1 - Elsner, Birgit T1 - Longitudinal reciprocity between theory of mind and aggression in middle childhood JF - International Journal of Behavioral Development N2 - Theory of mind is one of the most important cognitive factors in social information-processing, and deficits in theory of mind have been linked to aggressive behavior in childhood. The present longitudinal study investigated reciprocal links between theory of mind and two forms of aggression – physical and relational – in middle childhood with three data waves over 3 years. Theory of mind was assessed by participants’ responses to cartoons, and physical and relational aggression were assessed through teacher reports in a community sample of 1657 children (mean age at Time 1: 8 years). Structural equation modeling analyses showed that theory of mind was a negative predictor of subsequent physical and relational aggression, both from Time 1 to Time 2 as well as from Time 2 to Time 3. Moreover, relational aggression was a negative predictor of theory of mind from Time 1 to Time 2. There were no significant gender or age differences in the tested pathways. The results suggest that reciprocal and negative longitudinal relations exist between children’s theory of mind and aggressive behavior. Our study extends current knowledge about the development of such relations across middle childhood. KW - Aggressive behavior KW - theory of mind KW - middle childhood KW - longitudinal study KW - relational aggression KW - physical aggression Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025417727875 SN - 0165-0254 SN - 1464-0651 VL - 42 IS - 2 SP - 257 EP - 266 PB - Sage Publ. CY - London ER - TY - THES A1 - Rettig, Anja T1 - Learning to read in German BT - eye movements and the perceptual span of German beginning readers and their relation to reading motivation BT - Blickbewegungen und die perzentuelle Lesespanne von deutschsprachigen Leseanfängern und der Zusammenhang zur Lesemotivation N2 - In the present dissertation, the development of eye movement behavior and the perceptual span of German beginning readers was investigated in Grades 1 to 3 (Study 1) and longitudinally within a one-year time interval (Study 2), as well as in relation to intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation (Study 3). The presented results are intended to fill the gap of only sparse information on young readers’ eye movements and completely missing information on German young readers’ perceptual span and its development. On the other hand, reading motivation data have been scrutinized with respect to reciprocal effects on reading comprehension but not with respect to more immediate, basic cognitive processing (e.g., word decoding) that is indicated by different eye movement measures. Based on a longitudinal study design, children in Grades 1–3 participated in a moving window reading experiment with eye movement recordings in two successive years. All children were participants of a larger longitudinal study on intrapersonal developmental risk factors in childhood and adolescence (PIER study). Motivation data and other psychometric reading data were collected during individual inquiries and tests at school. Data analyses were realized in three separate studies that focused on different but related aspects of reading and perceptual span development. Study 1 presents the first cross-sectional report on the perceptual span of beginning German readers. The focus was on reading rate changes in Grades 1 to 3 and on the issue of the onset of the perceptual span development and its dependence on basic foveal reading processes. Study 2 presents a successor of Study 1 providing first longitudinal data of the perceptual span in elementary school children. It also includes information on the stability of observed and predicted reading rates and perceptual span sizes and introduces a new measure of the perceptual span based on nonlinear mixed-effects models. Another issue addressed in this study is the longitudinal between-group comparison of slower and faster readers which refers to the detection of developmental patterns. Study 3 includes longitudinal reading motivation data and investigates the relation between different eye movement measures including perceptual span and intrinsic as well as extrinsic reading motivation. In Study 1, a decelerated increase in reading rate was observed between Grades 1 to 3. Grade effects were also reported for saccade length, refixation probability, and different fixation duration measures. With higher grade, mean saccade length increased, whereas refixation probability, first-fixation duration, gaze duration, and total reading time decreased. Perceptual span development was indicated by an increase in window size effects with grade level. Grade level differences with respect to window size effects were stronger between Grades 2 and 3 than between Grades 1 and 2. These results were replicated longitudinally in Study 2. Again, perceptual span size significantly changed between Grades 2 and 3, but not between Grades 1 and 2 or Grades 3 and 4. Observed and predicted reading rates were found to be highly stable after first grade, whereas stability of perceptual span was only moderate for all grade levels. Group differences between slower and faster readers in Year 1 remained observable in Year 2 showing a pattern of stable achievement differences rather than a compensatory pattern. Between Grades 2 and 3, between-group differences in reading rate even increased resulting in a Matthew effect. A similar effect was observed for perceptual span development between Grades 3 and 4. Finally, in Study 3, significant relations between beginning readers’ eye movements and their reading motivation were observed. In both years of measurement, higher intrinsic reading motivation was related to more skilled eye movement patterns as indicated by short fixations, longer saccades, and higher reading rates. In Year 2, intrinsic reading motivation was also significantly and negatively correlated with refixation probability. These correlational patterns were confirmed in cross-sectional linear models controlling for grade level and reading amount and including both reading motivation measures, extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. While there were significant positive relations between intrinsic reading motivation and word decoding as indicated by the above stated eye movement measures, extrinsic reading motivation only predicted variance in eye movements in Year 2 (significant for fixation durations and reading rate), with a consistently opposite pattern of effects as compared to intrinsic reading motivation. Finally, longitudinal effects of Year 1 intrinsic reading motivation on Year 2 word decoding were observed for gaze duration, total reading time, refixation probability, and perceptual span within cross-lagged panel models. These effects were reciprocal because all eye movement measures significantly predicted variance in intrinsic reading motivation. Extrinsic reading motivation in Year 1 did not affect any eye movement measure in Year 2, and vice versa, except for a significant, negative relation with perceptual span. Concluding, the present dissertation demonstrates that largest gains in reading development in terms of eye movement changes are observable between Grades 1 and 2. Together with the observed pattern of stable differences between slower and faster readers and a widening achievement gap between Grades 2 and 3 for reading rate, these results underline the importance of the first year(s) of formal reading instruction. The development of the perceptual span lags behind as it is most apparent between Grades 2 and 3. This suggests that efficient parafoveal processing presupposes a certain degree of foveal reading proficiency (e.g., word decoding). Finally, this dissertation demonstrates that intrinsic reading motivation—but not extrinsic motivation—effectively supports the development of skilled reading. N2 - In der vorliegenden Dissertation wurde die Entwicklung der Blickbewegungen und der perzeptuellen Lesespanne von deutschsprachigen Leseanfängern in den Klassenstufen 1–3 im Querschnitt (Studie 1) als auch im Längsschnitt innerhalb eines Jahres (Studie 2) sowie hinsichtlich des Zusammenhangs mit der intrinsischen und der extrinsischen Lesemotivation (Studie 3) untersucht. Die Ergebnisse dieser Arbeit stellen einen bedeutsamen empirischen Beitrag zur ansonsten verhältnismäßig eher spärlichen empirisch-experimentellen Forschung zur frühen Leseentwicklung dar und liefern erste Erkenntnisse über die perzeptuelle Spanne von jungen deutschsprachigen Lesern. Des Weiteren wurde Neuland betreten, indem Blickdaten im Zusammenhang mit Lesemotivationsdaten ausgewertet wurden. Während es umfangreiche Forschungsarbeiten zum reziproken Zusammenhang zwischen Lesemotivation und dem Leserverstehen gibt, ist kaum etwas zu wechselseitigen Lesemotivationseffekten in Bezug auf basale kognitive Prozesse (z.B. die Wort-Dekodierung), wie sie durch verschiedene Blickbewegungsmaße indiziert werden, bekannt. Auf Grundlage eines längsschnittlichen Untersuchungsdesigns nahmen Kinder der Klassenstufen 1–3 in zwei aufeinanderfolgenden Jahren an einem Moving-Window-Leseexperiment mit manipuliertem Text teil. Alle Kinder waren Teilnehmer einer größeren Längsschnittstudie zur Untersuchung von intrapersonellen Risikofaktoren im Kindes- und Jugendalter (PIER-Studie). In individuellen Befragungen und Testungen in den Schulen wurden u.a. auch Lesemotivations- und andere psychometrische Lesedaten erhoben. Die im Labor erfassten Blickdaten wurden zusammen mit diesen psychometrischen Daten im Rahmen von drei separaten Studien ausgewertet, die jeweils verschiedene, jedoch miteinander in Bezug stehende Aspekte der Lese- und Lesespannen-Entwicklung untersuchen. Studie 1 präsentiert einen ersten querschnittlichen Bericht zur perzeptuellen Lesespanne von deutschsprachigen Leseanfängern. Hierbei lag der Fokus auf Veränderungen der Leserate in den Klassenstufen 1–3 und auf der Frage, wann die Entwicklung der perzeptuellen Spanne beginnt und inwiefern diese Entwicklung von basalen fovealen Leseprozessen abhängig ist. Studie 2 stellt eine Folgeuntersuchung dar, die erste Längsschnittdaten zur Entwicklung der perzeptuellen Lesespanne bei Grundschulkindern liefert. Untersucht wurden desweiteren die Stabilität der beobachteten und vorhergesagten Leserate als auch der perzeptuellen Lesespanne. In diesem Zusammenhang wird ein neues Spannenmaß vorgestellt, welches auf nicht-linearen gemischten Modellen basiert. Eine weitere Fragestellung der Studie ist der längsschnittliche Gruppenvergleich von langsameren und schnelleren Lesern, welcher auf die Entdeckung von Entwicklungsmustern abzielt. Studie 3 inkludiert längsschnittliche Lesemotivatonsdaten und untersucht die Beziehung zwischen verschiedenen Blickbewegungsmaßen einschließlich der perzeptuellen Lesespanne und der intrinsischen als auch extrinsischen Lesemotivation unter Berücksichtigung der Lesehäufigkeit. In Hinblick auf die Leseentwicklung in Klassenstufe 1–3 wurde ein zwischen den Klassenstufen abnehmender sukzessiver Anstieg in der Leserate beobachtet. Klassenstufeneffekte wurden außerdem berichtet für die Sakkadenlänge, die Refixationswahrscheinlichkeit und für verschiedene Fixationsdauermaße. Mit höherer Klassenstufe stieg die mittlere Sakkadenlänge, wohingegen die Refixationswahrscheinlichkeit, die Dauer der ersten Fixation auf einem Wort, die Blickdauer im sogenannten First-Pass und die Gesamtlesedauer von Worten abnahmen. Die Entwicklung der perzeptuellen Lesespanne wurde ersichtlich durch einen Anstieg von Fenstergrößen-Effekten mit steigender Klassenstufe. Der Unterschied zwischen den Klassenstufen im Hinblick auf Fenstergrößen-Effekte war größer zwischen Klasse 2 und 3 als zwischen den Klassen 1 und 2. Diese Ergebnisse wurden längsschnittlich repliziert in Studie 2. Wieder zeigte sich ein signifikanter Unterschied in der perzeptuellen Lesespanne zwischen Klassenstufe 2 und 3, jedoch nicht zwischen Klassenstufe 1 und 2 oder Klassenstufe 3 und 4. Die beobachtete und die vorhergesagte Leserate waren hoch stabil jenseits der ersten Klasse, wohingegen für die perzeptuelle Lesespanne für alle Klassenstufen nur eine moderate Stabilität gefunden wurde. Gruppenunterschiede zwischen langsameren und schnelleren Lesern im ersten Untersuchungsjahr wurden auch im zweiten Untersuchungsjahr beobachtet. Dabei zeichnete sich ein Muster eher stabiler anstatt kompensatorischer Leistungsunterschiede ab. Zwischen Klassenstufe 2 und 3 gab es sogar einen Anstieg der Disparität zwischen den Gruppen für die Leserate. Es zeichnete sich also ein sogenannter Matthäus-Effekt ab. Ein ähnlicher Effekt wurde für die perzeptuelle Lesespanne zwischen Klassenstufe 3 und 4 beobachtet. Abschließend wurde in Studie 3 ein signifikanter Zusammenhang zwischen den Blickbewegungen von Leseanfängern und ihrer Lesemotivation gefunden. In beiden Erhebungsjahren, korrelierte eine höhere intrinsische Lesemotivation mit geübteren Blickbewegungsmustern, was sich in kürzeren Fixationen, längeren Sakkaden und höheren Leseraten zeigte sowie im zweiten Erhebungsjahr auch in kleineren Refixationswahrscheinlichkeiten. In querschnittlichen linearen Modellen erwies sich die intrinsiche Lesemotivation als signifikanter Prädiktor für die oben genannten Blickmaße, selbst wenn für Klassenstufe und Lesehäufigkeit kontrolliert wurde und beide Motivationsmaße, die intrinische und die extrinsische Motivation, gleichzeitig ins Modell aufgenommen wurden. Die extrinsische Lesemotivation erwies sich hingegen nur im zweiten Erhebungsjahr als signifikanter Prädiktor der verschiedenen Fixationsdauern und der Leserate, wobei das Effektmuster durchweg entgegengesetzt zu dem für die intrinsische Lesemotivation beobachteten war. Schließlich wurden in kreuzverzögerten Autoregressionsmodellen längsschnittliche Effekte der intrinisichen Lesemotivation auf verschiedene Blickbewegungsmaße (Blickdauer im First-Pass, Gesamtlesezeit, Refixationswahrscheinlichkeit und perzeptuelle Lesespanne) beobachet. Diese Effekte waren reziprok, da alle Blickbewegungsmaße auch signifikant Varianz in der intrinsischen Lesemotivation vorhergesagt haben. Im Gegensatz dazu gab es weder signifikante längsschnittliche Effekte der extrinsichen Lesemotivation auf das Blickverhalten noch in die Gegenrichtung signifikante Effekte von Blickbewegungsmaßen auf die extrinsische Lesemotivation, mit Ausnahme einer signifikanten negativen Beziehung zwischen der extrinsischen Lesemotivation und der Lesespanne. Zusammenfassend lassen sich folgende Erkenntnisse festhalten: Die aktuelle Dissertation zeigt auf, dass der größte Zuwachs bei der Leseentwicklung im Sinne von Blickbewegungsänderungen zwischen den Klassenstufen 1 und 2 zu beobachten ist. Zusammen mit dem beobachteten Muster zeitlich stabiler Gruppenunterschiede zwischen langsameren und schnelleren Lesern und dem größer werdenden Leistungsabstand zwischen Klassenstufe 2 und 3 für das Maß der Leserate unterstreichen die Ergebnisse die Bedeutsamkeit des (der) ersten Jahre(s) formaler Leseinstruktion. Die Entwicklung der perzeptuellen Lesespanne ist verzögert, da sie am deutlichsten zwischen den Klassenstufen 2 und 3 sichtbar wird. Dies legt die Schlussfolgerung nah, dass effiziente parafoveale Verarbeitung einen gewissen Grad an fovealer Lesefertigkeit (d.h. basale Wortdekodierfähigkeiten) erfordert. Schließlich liefert die aktuelle Dissertation auch empirische Belege dafür, dass die intrinsische—aber nicht die extrinsische—Lesemotivation effektiv die Leseentwicklung unterstützt. T2 - Lesen Lernen im Deutschen KW - eye movements KW - perceptual span KW - reading development KW - beginning readers KW - German KW - moving window KW - longitudinal study KW - reading motivation KW - Blickbewegungen KW - perzentuelle Lesespanne KW - Leseentwicklung KW - Leseanfänger KW - Deutsch KW - Moving Window KW - Längsschnittstudie KW - Lesemotivation Y1 - 2021 ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Rohlf, Helena L. A1 - Holl, Anna K. A1 - Kirsch, Fabian A1 - Krahé, Barbara A1 - Elsner, Birgit T1 - Longitudinal Links between Executive Function, Anger, and Aggression in Middle Childhood JF - Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience N2 - Previous research has indicated that executive function (EF) is negatively associated with aggressive behavior in childhood. However, there is a lack of longitudinal studies that have examined the effect of deficits in EF on aggression over time and taken into account different forms and functions of aggression at the same time. Furthermore, only few studies have analyzed the role of underlying variables that may explain the association between EF and aggression. The present study examined the prospective paths between EF and different forms (physical and relational) and functions (reactive and proactive) of aggression. The habitual experience of anger was examined as a potential underlying mechanism of the link between EF and aggression, because the tendency to get angry easily has been found to be both a consequence of deficits in EF and a predictor of aggression. The study included 1,652 children (between 6 and 11 years old at the first time point), who were followed over three time points (T1, T2, and T3) covering 3 years. At T1, a latent factor of EF comprised measures of planning, rated via teacher reports, as well as inhibition, set shifting, and working-memory updating, assessed experimentally. Habitual anger experience was assessed via parent reports at T1 and T2. The forms and functions of aggression were measured via teacher reports at all three time points. Structural equation modeling revealed that EF at T1 predicted physical, relational, and reactive aggression at T3, but was unrelated to proactive aggression at T3. Furthermore, EF at T1 was indirectly linked to physical aggression at T3, mediated through habitual anger experience at T2. The results indicate that deficits in EF influence the later occurrence of aggression in middle childhood, and the tendency to get angry easily mediates this relation. KW - executive function KW - anger KW - relational aggression KW - reactive aggression KW - proactive aggression KW - childhood KW - longitudinal study Y1 - 2018 U6 - https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00027 SN - 1662-5153 VL - 12 IS - 27 SP - 1 EP - 14 PB - Frontiers Research Foundation CY - Lausanne ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Tetzner, Julia A1 - Becker, Michael T1 - How Being an Optimist Makes a Difference: The Protective Role of Optimism in Adolescents' Adjustment to Parental Separation JF - Social psychological and personality science N2 - This longitudinal study of N = 1,566 adolescents investigated the protective role of optimism in adjustment to parental separation, focusing on two salient challenges faced by adolescents, namely academic achievement and self-esteem. Based on latent change models, the results indicated associations between parental separation and short-term declines in academic achievement as well as short-term and longer term declines in self-esteem. Although optimism in general showed positive associations with academic achievement and self-esteem, its role as a protective factor proved to be particularly important for academic achievement in adjustment following parental separation. KW - parental separation KW - optimism KW - academic achievement KW - self-esteem KW - longitudinal study Y1 - 2015 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614559605 SN - 1948-5506 SN - 1948-5514 VL - 6 IS - 3 SP - 325 EP - 333 PB - Sage Publ. CY - Thousand Oaks ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Krahé, Barbara A1 - Busching, Robert T1 - Breaking the Vicious Cycle of Media Violence Use and Aggression: A Test of Intervention Effects Over 30 Months JF - Psychology of violence N2 - Objective: This study examined the sustained efficacy of a media violence intervention in reducing media violence use, normative acceptance of aggression, and aggressive behavior in adolescents. It used an experimental design to evaluate the effects of the intervention over a period of 30 months. Method: N = 627 German 7th and 8th graders were assigned to a 5-week school-based intervention to reduce media violence use or to a no-intervention control group. Media violence use, normative acceptance of aggression, and aggressive behavior were measured 3 months before the intervention (T1), 7 months post intervention (T2), and at 2 follow-ups 18 (T3) and 30 (T4) months after the intervention. This article focuses on the findings from the 2 follow-ups. Results: Controlling for baseline levels and various demographic covariates, media violence use at T2, T3, and T4 and self-reported physical aggression at T3 were significantly lower in the intervention group, and the indirect path from the intervention to T3 aggression via T2 media violence use was significant. Lower T2 media violence use predicted lower T3 normative acceptance of aggression among participants with lower initial aggression. No effects on nonviolent media use and relational aggression were observed. Conclusion: The findings show that a short class-based intervention can produce lasting changes in media violence use that are linked to a decrease in aggression. KW - aggression KW - experimental evaluation KW - intervention KW - longitudinal study KW - media violence Y1 - 2015 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036627 SN - 2152-0828 SN - 2152-081X VL - 5 IS - 2 SP - 217 EP - 226 PB - American Psychological Association CY - Washington ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Kirsch, Fabian A1 - Rohlf, Helena L. A1 - Krahé, Barbara T1 - Measuring anger regulation in middle childhood through behavioural observation: a longitudinal validation JF - European journal of developmental psychology N2 - Learning to regulate anger is an important task in childhood development, as maladaptive anger regulation has been linked to a variety of problems, including aggression and social rejection. To assess anger regulation in situ, in a previous study we developed a behavioural observation measure and demonstrated its cross-sectional construct and criterion validity in a sample of 599 children with a mean age of 8.1years. The present study further validated the measure by demonstrating its predictive validity. About 10months after the behavioural observation, participants were asked to imagine two anger-eliciting situations and report what they would do to get rid of their anger. Observed anger regulation strategies at T1 correlated significantly with self-reported regulatory behaviour at T2, suggesting that the behavioural observation measure is an ecologically valid approach for assessing anger regulation in middle childhood. KW - Anger regulation KW - observation KW - childhood KW - validation KW - longitudinal study Y1 - 2015 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2015.1101375 SN - 1740-5629 SN - 1740-5610 VL - 12 IS - 6 SP - 718 EP - 727 PB - Routledge, Taylor & Francis Group CY - Abingdon ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Krahé, Barbara A1 - Busching, Robert T1 - Interplay of normative beliefs and behavior in developmental patterns of physical and relational aggression in adolescence: a four-wave longitudinal study JF - Frontiers in psychology N2 - In a longitudinal study with N = 1,854 adolescents from Germany, we investigated patterns of change and gender differences in physical and relational aggression in relation to normative beliefs about these two forms of aggression. Participants, whose mean age was 13 years at T1, completed self-report measures of physically and relationally aggressive behavior and indicated their normative approval of both forms of aggression at four data waves separated by 12-month intervals. Boys scored higher than did girls on both forms of aggression, but the gender difference was more pronounced for physical aggression. Physical aggression decreased and relational aggression increased over the four data waves in both gender groups. The normative acceptance of both forms of aggression decreased over time, with a greater decrease for the approval of physical aggression. In both gender groups, normative approval of relational aggression prospectively predicted relational aggression across all data waves, and the normative approval of physical aggression predicted physically aggressive behavior at the second and third data waves. A reciprocal reinforcement of aggressive norms and behavior was found for both forms of aggression. The findings are discussed as supporting a social information processing perspective on developmental patterns of change in physical and relational aggression in adolescence. KW - physical aggression KW - relational aggression KW - aggressive norms KW - adolescence KW - gender KW - longitudinal study KW - Germany Y1 - 2014 U6 - https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01146 SN - 1664-1078 VL - 5 PB - Frontiers Research Foundation CY - Lausanne ER - TY - JOUR A1 - Buchmann, Arlette F. A1 - Blomeyer, Dorothea A1 - Jennen-Steinmetz, Christine A1 - Schmidt, Martin H. A1 - Esser, Günter A1 - Banaschewski, Tobias A1 - Laucht, Manfred T1 - Early smoking onset may promise initial pleasurable sensations and later addiction JF - Addiction biology N2 - There is converging evidence suggesting a particular susceptibility to the addictive properties of nicotine among adolescents. The aim of the current study was to prospectively ascertain the relationship between age at first cigarette and initial smoking experiences, and to examine the combined effects of these characteristics of adolescent smoking behavior on adult smoking. It was hypothesized that the association between earlier age at first cigarette and later development of nicotine dependence may, at least in part, be attributable to differences in experiencing pleasurable early smoking sensations. Data were drawn from the participants of the Mannheim Study of Children at Risk, an ongoing epidemiological cohort study from birth to adulthood. Structured interviews at age 15, 19 and 22 years were conducted to assess the age at first cigarette, early smoking experiences and current smoking behavior in 213 young adults. In addition, the participants completed the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence. Adolescents who smoked their first cigarette at an earlier age reported more pleasurable sensations from the cigarette, and they were more likely to be regular smokers at age 22. The age at first cigarette also predicted the number of cigarettes smoked and dependence at age 22. Thus, both the age of first cigarette and the pleasure experienced from the cigarette independently predicted aspects of smoking at age 22. KW - Adolescence KW - age at first cigarette KW - dependence KW - early smoking experiences KW - longitudinal study KW - pleasurable smoking sensations Y1 - 2013 U6 - https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00377.x SN - 1369-1600 VL - 18 IS - 6 SP - 947 EP - 954 PB - Wiley-Blackwell CY - Hoboken ER -