@article{RolfsDambacherCavanagh2013, author = {Rolfs, Martin and Dambacher, Michael and Cavanagh, Patrick}, title = {Visual adaptation of the perception of causality}, series = {Current biology}, volume = {23}, journal = {Current biology}, number = {3}, publisher = {Cell Press}, address = {Cambridge}, issn = {0960-9822}, doi = {10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.017}, pages = {250 -- 254}, year = {2013}, abstract = {We easily recover the causal properties of visual events, enabling us to understand and predict changes in the physical world. We see a tennis racket hitting a ball and sense that it caused the ball to fly over the net; we may also have an eerie but equally compelling experience of causality if the streetlights turn on just as we slam our car's door. Both perceptual [1] and cognitive [2] processes have been proposed to explain these spontaneous inferences, but without decisive evidence one way or the other, the question remains wide open [3-8]. Here, we address this long-standing debate using visual adaptation-a powerful tool to uncover neural populations that specialize in the analysis of specific visual features [9-12]. After prolonged viewing of causal collision events called "launches" [1], subsequently viewed events were judged more often as noncausal. These negative aftereffects of exposure to collisions are spatially localized in retinotopic coordinates, the reference frame shared by the retina and visual cortex. They are not explained by adaptation to other stimulus features and reveal visual routines in retinotopic cortex that detect and adapt to cause and effect in simple collision stimuli.}, language = {en} } @article{RolfsOhl2011, author = {Rolfs, Martin and Ohl, Sven}, title = {Visual suppression in the superior colliculus around the time of microsaccades}, series = {Journal of neurophysiology}, volume = {105}, journal = {Journal of neurophysiology}, number = {1}, publisher = {American Chemical Society}, address = {Bethesda}, issn = {0022-3077}, doi = {10.1152/jn.00862.2010}, pages = {1 -- 3}, year = {2011}, abstract = {Miniature eye movements jitter the retinal image unceasingly, raising the question of how perceptual continuity is achieved during visual fixation. Recent work discovered suppression of visual bursts in the superior colliculus around the time of microsaccades, tiny jerks of the eyes that support visual perception while gaze is fixed. This finding suggests that corollary discharge, supporting visual stability when rapid eye movements drastically shift the retinal image, may also exist for the smallest saccades.}, language = {en} } @article{LaubrockKlieglRolfsetal.2010, author = {Laubrock, Jochen and Kliegl, Reinhold and Rolfs, Martin and Engbert, Ralf}, title = {When do microsaccades follow spatial attention?}, issn = {1943-3921}, doi = {10.3758/APP.72.3.683}, year = {2010}, abstract = {Following up on an exchange about the relation between microsaccades and spatial attention (Horowitz, Fencsik, Fine, Yurgenson, \& Wolfe, 2007; Horowitz, Fine, Fencsik, Yurgenson, \& Wolfe, 2007; Laubrock, Engbert, Rolfs, \& Kliegl, 2007), we examine the effects of selection criteria and response modality. We show that for Posner cuing with saccadic responses, microsaccades go with attention in at least 75\% of cases (almost 90\% if probability matching is assumed) when they are first (or only) microsaccades in the cue target interval and when they occur between 200 and 400 msec after the cue. The relation between spatial attention and the direction of microsaccades drops to chance level for unselected microsaccades collected during manual-response conditions. Analyses of data from four cross-modal cuing experiments demonstrate an above-chance, intermediate link for visual cues, but no systematic relation for auditory cues. Thus, the link between spatial attention and direction of microsaccades depends on the experimental condition and time of occurrence, but it can be very strong.}, language = {en} } @article{Rolfs2009, author = {Rolfs, Martin}, title = {Microsaccades : small steps on a long way}, issn = {0042-6989}, doi = {10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.010}, year = {2009}, abstract = {Contrary to common wisdom, fixations are a dynamically rich behavior, composed of continual, miniature eye movements, of which microsaccades are the most salient component. Over the last few years, interest in these small movements has risen dramatically, driven by both neurophysiological and psychophysical results and by advances in techniques, analysis, and modeling of eye movements. The field has a long history but a significant portion of the earlier work has gone missing in the current literature, in part, as a result of the collapse of the field in the 1980s that followed a series of discouraging results. The present review compiles 60 years of work demonstrating the unique contribution of microsaccades to visual and oculomotor function. Specifically, the review covers the contribution of microsaccades to (1) the control of fixation position, (2) the reduction of perceptual fading and the continuity of perception, (3) the generation of synchronized visual transients, (4) visual acuity, (5) scanning of small spatial regions, (6) shifts of spatial attention, (7) resolving perceptual ambiguities in the face of multistable perception, as well as several other functions. The accumulated evidence demonstrates that microsaccades serve both perceptual and oculomotor goals and although in some cases their contribution is neither necessary nor unique, microsaccades are a malleable tool conveniently employed by the visual system.}, language = {en} } @misc{DambacherRolfsGoellneretal.2009, author = {Dambacher, Michael and Rolfs, Martin and G{\"o}llner, Kristin and Kliegl, Reinhold and Jacobs, Arthur M.}, title = {Event-related potentials reveal rapid verification of predicted visual input}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-44953}, year = {2009}, abstract = {Human information processing depends critically on continuous predictions about upcoming events, but the temporal convergence of expectancy-based top-down and input-driven bottom-up streams is poorly understood. We show that, during reading, event-related potentials differ between exposure to highly predictable and unpredictable words no later than 90 ms after visual input. This result suggests an extremely rapid comparison of expected and incoming visual information and gives an upper temporal bound for theories of top-down and bottom-up interactions in object recognition.}, language = {en} } @article{KlieglRolfsLaubrocketal.2009, author = {Kliegl, Reinhold and Rolfs, Martin and Laubrock, Jochen and Engbert, Ralf}, title = {Microsaccadic modulation of response times in spatial attention tasks}, issn = {0340-0727}, doi = {10.1007/s00426-008-0202-2}, year = {2009}, language = {en} } @article{BoettcherRolfsKliegletal.2009, author = {B{\"o}ttcher, Heiko and Rolfs, Martin and Kliegl, Reinhold and Ihle, Wolfgang}, title = {Inattentional blindness and change blindness bei Jungen mit ADHS}, year = {2009}, language = {en} } @misc{KlieglRolfsLaubrocketal.2009, author = {Kliegl, Reinhold and Rolfs, Martin and Laubrock, Jochen and Engbert, Ralf}, title = {Microsaccadic Modulation of Response Times in Spatial Attention Tasks}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-57098}, year = {2009}, abstract = {Covert shifts of attention are usually reflected in RT differences between responses to valid and invalid cues in the Posner spatial attention task. Such inferences about covert shifts of attention do not control for microsaccades in the cue target interval. We analyzed the effects of microsaccade orientation on RTs in four conditions, crossing peripheral visual and auditory cues with peripheral visual and auditory discrimination targets. Reaction time was generally faster on trials without microsaccades in the cue-target interval. If microsaccades occurred, the target-location congruency of the last microsaccade in the cuetarget interval interacted in a complex way with cue validity. For valid visual cues, irrespective of whether the discrimination target was visual or auditory, target-congruent microsaccades delayed RT. For invalid cues, target-incongruent microsaccades facilitated RTs for visual target discrimination, but delayed RT for auditory target discrimination. No reliable effects on RT were associated with auditory cues or with the first microsaccade in the cue-target interval. We discuss theoretical implications on the relation about spatial attention and oculomotor processes.}, language = {en} } @article{RolfsKlieglEngbert2008, author = {Rolfs, Martin and Kliegl, Reinhold and Engbert, Ralf}, title = {Toward a model of microsaccade generation : the case of microsaccadic inhibition}, issn = {1534-7362}, doi = {10.1167/8.11.5}, year = {2008}, language = {en} } @article{ThielRomanoKurthsetal.2008, author = {Thiel, Marco and Romano, Maria Carmen and Kurths, J{\"u}rgen and Rolfs, Martin and Kliegl, Reinhold}, title = {Generating surrogates from recurrences}, issn = {1364-503X}, year = {2008}, abstract = {In this paper, we present an approach to recover the dynamics from recurrences of a system and then generate (multivariate) twin surrogate (TS) trajectories. In contrast to other approaches, such as the linear-like surrogates, this technique produces surrogates which correspond to an independent copy of the underlying system, i.e. they induce a trajectory of the underlying system visiting the attractor in a different way. We show that these surrogates are well suited to test for complex synchronization, which makes it possible to systematically assess the reliability of synchronization analyses. We then apply the TS to study binocular fixational movements and find strong indications that the fixational movements of the left and right eye are phase synchronized. This result indicates that there might be only one centre in the brain that produces the fixational movements in both eyes or a close link between the two centres.}, language = {en} }