@article{SchwarzKisselePfeiletal.1997, author = {Schwarz, D. and Kissele, P. and Pfeil, Wolfgang and Pisch, S. and Bornscheuer, U. and Schmid, Rolf D.}, title = {Evidence that nonbilayer phase probensity of the membrane is important for thr side chain cleavage of cytochrome P450SCC (CYP11A1)}, year = {1997}, language = {en} } @article{FeudelJansenKurthsetal.1997, author = {Feudel, Ulrike and Jansen, Wolfgang and Kurths, J{\"u}rgen and Schwarz, Udo and Voss, Henning U.}, title = {Solar variability : simple models and proxy data}, isbn = {4-274-90187-4}, year = {1997}, language = {en} } @article{SchwarzIschebeck2003, author = {Schwarz, Wolfgang and Ischebeck, A.}, title = {On the relative speed account of number-size interference effects in comparative judgments of numerals}, year = {2003}, language = {en} } @article{Schwarz2003, author = {Schwarz, Wolfgang}, title = {Stovhastic cascade processes as a model of multi-stage concurrent information procesing}, year = {2003}, language = {en} } @article{SchwarzKeus2004, author = {Schwarz, Wolfgang and Keus, I. M.}, title = {Moving the eyes along the mental number line : comparing SNARC effects with saccadic and manual responses}, year = {2004}, abstract = {Bimanual parity judgments about numerically small (large) digits are faster with the left (right) hand, even though parity is unrelated to numerical magnitude per se (the SNARC effect; Dehaene, Bossini, \& Giraux, 1993). According to one model, this effect reflects a space-related representation of numerical magnitudes (mental number line) with a genuine left-to-right orientation. Alternatively, it may simply reflect an overlearned motor association between numbers and manual responses-as, for example, on typewriters or computer keyboards-in which case it should be weaker or absent with effectors whose horizontal response component is less systematically associated with individual numbers. Two experiments involving comparisons of saccadic and manual parity judgment tasks clearly support the first view; they also establish a vertical SNARC effect, suggesting that our magnitude representation resembles a number map, rather than a number line}, language = {en} } @article{KeusJenksSchwarz2005, author = {Keus, I. M. and Jenks, C. and Schwarz, Wolfgang}, title = {Psychophysiological evidence that the SNARC effect has a functional locus in a response selection stage}, year = {2005}, abstract = {When participants judge the parity of visually presented digits, left-hand responses are faster for numerically small numbers, whereas right-hand responses are faster for large numbers [SNARC effect; S. Dehaene, S. Bossini, P. Giraux, The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 122, (1993) 371-396]. The present study aimed to find more direct evidence for the functional locus of this effect by recording brain waves while participants performed speeded parity judgments giving manual responses. Our results show clear and robust SNARC effects in the response-locked event-related potentials (ERPs) compared to the stimulus-locked ERPs, confirming that the SNARC effect arises during response-related rather than stimulus-related processing stages. Further analyses of lateralized readiness potentials strongly suggest that the SNARC effect begins to emerge in a response-related stage prior to response preparation and execution, more specifically, in a response selection stage. (c) 2005 Elsevier B.V All rights reserved}, language = {en} } @article{SchwarzMueller2005, author = {Schwarz, Wolfgang and M{\"u}ller, Dana}, title = {Spatial associatiosn in number-related tasks : a comparison of manual and pedal responses}, year = {2005}, abstract = {Bimanual parity judgments of numerically small (large) digits are faster with the left (right) hand (the SNARC effect; Dehaene, Bossini, \& Giraux, 1993). According to one explanation, this effect is culturally derived and reflects ontogenetic influences such as the direction of written language; it might therefore be limited to, or at least be larger with, pairs of lateralized effectors which are instrumental to the production and comprehension of written language. We report two experiments which test for SNARC effects with pedal responses, and compare these effects to manual results. Pedal responses yielded highly systematic SNARC effects; furthermore, these effects did not differ from manual SNARC effects, These results argue against accounts in which the SNARC effect is specific for effectors that are habitually associated with the production or comprehension of written language}, language = {en} } @article{KeusSchwarz2005, author = {Keus, I. M. and Schwarz, Wolfgang}, title = {Searching for the functional locus of the SNARC effect : evidence for a response-related origin}, year = {2005}, language = {en} } @article{DeBleserSchwarzBurchert2006, author = {De Bleser, Ria and Schwarz, Wolfgang and Burchert, Frank}, title = {Quantitative neurosyntactic analyses : the final word?}, issn = {0093-934X}, doi = {10.1016/j.bandl.2005.06.010}, year = {2006}, language = {en} } @article{Schwarz2006, author = {Schwarz, Wolfgang}, title = {On the relation between the redundant signals effect and temporal order judgments: Parametric data and a new model}, year = {2006}, abstract = {Paradigms used to study the time course of the redundant signals effect (RSE; J. O. Miller, 1986) and temporal order judgments (TOJs) share many important similarities and address related questions concerning the time course of sensory processing. The author of this article proposes and tests a new aggregate diffusion-based model to quantitatively explain both the RSE and TOJs and the relationship between them. Parametric data (13 stimulus onset asynchronies) from an experiment with pairs of visual stimuli (626-nm LEDs) confirm that, relative to central signals (3 degrees), peripheral signals (35 degrees) yield slower reaction times, more strongly modulated RSE time-course functions, and flatter TOJ psychometric functions. All of these qualitative features are well captured, even in quantitative detail, by the aggregate diffusion model.}, language = {en} }