@article{ArndtEsserWeirichetal.2015, author = {Arndt, Larissa R. and Esser, G{\"u}nter and Weirich, Sebastian and Oelsner, Henriette and Ebersbach, Georg and Bengner, Thomas}, title = {Face Memory in Idiopathic Parkinson's Disease Moderated by Sex and Encoding Duration}, series = {Zeitschrift f{\"u}r Neuropsychologie}, volume = {26}, journal = {Zeitschrift f{\"u}r Neuropsychologie}, number = {2}, publisher = {Hogrefe}, address = {Bern}, issn = {1016-264X}, doi = {10.1024/1016-264X/a000148}, pages = {109 -- 120}, year = {2015}, abstract = {We examined face memory deficits in patients with Idiopathic Parkinson's disease (IPD) with specific regard to the moderating role of sex and the different memory processes involved. We tested short- and long-term face recognition memory in 18 nonclinical participants and 18 IPD-patients matched for sex, education and age. We varied the duration of item presentation (1, 5, 10s), the time of testing (immediately, 1hr, 24hrs) and the possibility to re-encode items. In accordance with earlier studies, we report face memory deficits in IPD. Moreover, our findings indicate that sex and encoding conditions may be important moderator variables. In contrast to healthy individuals, IPD-patients cannot gain from increasing duration of presentation. Furthermore, our results suggest that I PD leads to face memory deficits in women, only.}, language = {en} } @article{ArkesAbereggArpin2022, author = {Arkes, Hal R. and Aberegg, Scott K. and Arpin, Kevin A.}, title = {Analysis of physicians' probability estimates of a medical outcome based on a sequence of events}, series = {JAMA network open / American Medical Association}, volume = {5}, journal = {JAMA network open / American Medical Association}, number = {6}, publisher = {American Veterinary Medical Association}, address = {Chicago}, issn = {2574-3805}, doi = {10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.18804}, pages = {10}, year = {2022}, abstract = {IMPORTANCE The probability of a conjunction of 2 independent events is the product of the probabilities of the 2 components and therefore cannot exceed the probability of either component; violation of this basic law is called the conjunction fallacy. A common medical decision-making scenario involves estimating the probability of a final outcome resulting from a sequence of independent events; however, little is known about physicians' ability to accurately estimate the overall probability of success in these situations. OBJECTIVE To ascertain whether physicians are able to correctly estimate the overall probability of a medical outcome resulting from 2 independent events. DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS This survey study consisted of 3 separate substudies, in which 215 physicians were asked via internet-based survey to estimate the probability of success of each of 2 components of a diagnostic or prognostic sequence as well as the overall probability of success of the 2-step sequence. Substudy 1 was performed from April 2 to 4, 2021, substudy 2 from November 2 toll, 2021, and substudy 3 from May 13 to 19, 2021. All physicians were board certified or board eligible in the primary specialty germane to the substudy (ie, obstetrics and gynecology for substudies land 3 and pulmonology for substudy 2), were recruited from a commercial survey service, and volunteered to participate in the study. EXPOSURES Case scenarios presented in an online survey. MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Respondents were asked to provide their demographic information in addition to 3 probability estimates. The first substudy included a scenario describing a brow presentation discovered during labor; the 2 conjuncts were the probabilities that the brow presentation would resolve and that the delivery would be vaginal. The second substudy involved a diagnostic evaluation of an incidentally discovered pulmonary nodule; the 2 conjuncts were the probabilities that the patient had a malignant condition and that a technically successful transthoracic needle biopsy would reveal a malignant condition. The third substudy included a modification of the first substudy in an attempt to debias the conjunction fallacy prevalent in the first substudy. Respondents' own probability estimates of the individual events were used to calculate the mathematically correct conjunctive probability. RESULTS Among 215 respondents, the mean (SD) age was 54.0 (9.5) years; 142 respondents (66.0\%) were male. Data on race and ethnicity were not collected. A total of 168 physicians (78.1\%) estimated the probability of the 2-step sequence to be greater than the probability of at least 1 of the 2 component events. Compared with the product of their 2 estimated components, respondents overestimated the combined probability by 12.8\% (95\% CI, 9.6\%-16.1\%; P < .001) in substudy 1, 19.8\% (95\% Cl, 16.6\%-23.0\%; P < .001) in substudy 2, and 18.0\% (95\% CI, 13.4\%-22.5\%; P < .001) in substudy 3, results that were mathematically incoherent (ie, formally illogical and mathematically incorrect). CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE In this survey study of 215 physicians, respondents consistently overestimated the combined probability of 2 events compared with the probability calculated from their own estimates of the individual events. This biased estimation, consistent with the conjunction fallacy, may have substantial implications for diagnostic and prognostic decision-making.}, language = {en} } @article{ApelCangelosiEllisetal.2012, author = {Apel, Jens K. and Cangelosi, Angelo and Ellis, Rob and Goslin, Jeremy and Fischer, Martin H.}, title = {Object affordance influences instruction span}, series = {Experimental brain research}, volume = {223}, journal = {Experimental brain research}, number = {2}, publisher = {Springer}, address = {New York}, issn = {0014-4819}, doi = {10.1007/s00221-012-3251-0}, pages = {199 -- 206}, year = {2012}, abstract = {We measured memory span for assembly instructions involving objects with handles oriented to the left or right side. Right-handed participants remembered more instructions when objects' handles were spatially congruent with the hand used in forthcoming assembly actions. No such affordance-based memory benefit was found for left-handed participants. These results are discussed in terms of motor simulation as an embodied rehearsal mechanism.}, language = {en} } @article{Angerer2022, author = {Angerer, Marie-Luise}, title = {Nicht-bewusst}, publisher = {Turia + Kant}, address = {Berlin}, isbn = {978-3-98514-019-0}, pages = {119}, year = {2022}, abstract = {Die digitale Aufr{\"u}stung des Sozialen und Psychischen schreitet voran. Social media, verkabelte Infrastrukturen und autonome technische Artefakte bestimmen das Bild einer Gesellschaft, die in immer komplexeren Gef{\"u}gen agiert. Die zunehmende Pr{\"a}senz von nonhumanen Agenten (nicht nur in Film und Literatur), die Entwicklung von smarten Objekten und die sensortechnologische Ausstattung des menschlichen K{\"o}rpers und der Umwelt f{\"u}hren zu Kurzschl{\"u}ssen zwischen humaner und nonhumaner agency, die nicht mehr unbewusst, sondern nichtbewusst sind. Das Unbewusste der Psychoanalyse ist an ein humanes (sprachlich-symbolisch organisiertes) Subjekt gekoppelt, der Begriff »nicht«-bewusst verbindet hingegen technische, mentale und k{\"o}rperliche Prozesse und ist damit nicht mehr ausschließlich dem Menschen zuzuordnen. Doch wie ist dieses ›nichtbewusst‹ zu fassen: ist es etwas Zus{\"a}tzliches, ist es eine neue Zone, die sich zwischen unbewusst und bewusst schiebt, oder wird durch die Unterscheidung von unbewusst und bewusst dadurch insgesamt in Frage gestellt? Manches deutet auf die zweite Entwicklung hin. Denn sp{\"a}testens mit der Kybernetik werden technische und neuronale Prozesse zusammen gedacht. Gilbert Simondon hat die technische und humane Entwicklung als einen Prozess verstanden. Catherine Malabou f{\"u}hrt den Begriff des cerebral nonconscious ein, um das psychoanalytische Unbewusste in ein nichtbewusstes Gehirn {\"u}berzuf{\"u}hren, und N. Katherine Hayles spricht von nonconscious cognition, um das Zusammenwirken von neuronalen und technischen Prozessen zu benennen. Doch all diesen Unternehmungen, die diese (intensiven) Beziehungen von Gehirn und Maschine zu fassen suchen, fehlt ein wesentliches Moment der Verkopplung und psychischen Integration. Hierf{\"u}r wird der Begriff des Affektiven eingef{\"u}hrt, der als technischer Terminus die Bewegungen des Schließens, Unterbrechens und {\"U}bersetzens zwischen human und nonhuman bezeichnet. Dadurch werden die Kurzschl{\"u}sse von psycho-technischen Prozessen als nichtbewusste Taktung von Bewegungs- und Zeitformationen fassbar.}, language = {de} } @article{AngeleSlatteryYangetal.2008, author = {Angele, Bernhard and Slattery, Timothy J. and Yang, Jinmian and Kliegl, Reinhold and Rayner, Keith}, title = {Parafoveal processing in reading : manipulating n+1 and n+2 previews simultaneously}, issn = {1350-6285}, doi = {10.1080/13506280802009704}, year = {2008}, abstract = {The boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975) with a novel preview manipulation was used to examine the extent of parafoveal processing of words to the right of fixation. Words n + 1 and n + 2 had either correct or incorrect previews prior to fixation (prior to crossing the boundary location). In addition, the manipulation utilized either a high or low frequency word in word n + 1 location on the assumption that it would be more likely that n + 2 preview effects could be obtained when word n + 1 was high frequency. The primary findings were that there was no evidence for a preview benefit for word n + 2 and no evidence for parafoveal-on-foveal effects when word n + 1 is at least four letters long. We discuss implications for models of eye-movement control in reading.}, language = {en} } @misc{AngeleSlatteryYangetal.2008, author = {Angele, Bernhard and Slattery, Timothy J. and Yang, Jinmian and Kliegl, Reinhold and Rayner, Keith}, title = {Parafoveal processing in reading: Manipulating n+1 and n+2 previews simultaneously}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-57128}, year = {2008}, abstract = {The boundary paradigm (Rayner, 1975) with a novel preview manipulation was used to examine the extent of parafoveal processing of words to the right of fixation. Words n+1 and n+2 had either correct or incorrect previews prior to fixation (prior to crossing the boundary location). In addition, the manipulation utilized either a high or low frequency word in word n+1 location on the assumption that it would be more likely that n+2 preview effects could be obtained when word n+1 was high frequency. The primary findings were that there was no evidence for a preview benefit for word n+2 and no evidence for parafoveal-on-foveal effects when word n+1 is at least four letters long. We discuss implications for models of eye-movement control in reading.}, language = {en} } @article{AndreeHeywoodSchwanetal.2018, author = {Andree, Kai and Heywood, John S. and Schwan, Mike and Wang, Zheng}, title = {A spatial model of cartel stability}, series = {Bulletin of economic research}, volume = {70}, journal = {Bulletin of economic research}, number = {3}, publisher = {Wiley}, address = {Hoboken}, issn = {0307-3378}, doi = {10.1111/boer.12149}, pages = {298 -- 311}, year = {2018}, abstract = {We uniquely introduce convex production costs into a cartel model involving spatial price discrimination. We demonstrate that greater convexity improves cartel stability and that for sufficient convexity first best locations will be adopted. We show that allowing locations to vary over the game reduces cartel stability but that greater convexity continues to improve that stability. Moreover, when the degree of convexity does not support the first best collusive locations, other collusive locations exist that require less stability and these may either increase or decrease social welfare relative to competition. Critically, these locations that require less stability are more dispersed in sharp contrast to the known result assuming linear production costs.}, language = {en} } @article{AndersonSuzukiSwingetal.2017, author = {Anderson, Craig A. and Suzuki, Kanae and Swing, Edward L. and Groves, Christopher L. and Gentile, Douglas A. and Prot, Sara and Lam, Chun Pan and Sakamoto, Akira and Horiuchi, Yukiko and Krah{\´e}, Barbara and Jelic, Margareta and Wei Liuqing, and Toma, Roxana and Warburton, Wayne A. and Zhang, Xue-Min and Tajima, Sachi and Qing, Feng and Petrescu, Poesis}, title = {Media Violence and Other Aggression Risk Factors in Seven Nations}, series = {Personality and social psychology bulletin}, volume = {43}, journal = {Personality and social psychology bulletin}, publisher = {Sage Publ.}, address = {Thousand Oaks}, issn = {0146-1672}, doi = {10.1177/0146167217703064}, pages = {986 -- 998}, year = {2017}, abstract = {Cultural generality versus specificity of media violence effects on aggression was examined in seven countries (Australia, China, Croatia, Germany, Japan, Romania, the United States). Participants reported aggressive behaviors, media use habits, and several other known risk and protective factors for aggression. Across nations, exposure to violent screen media was positively associated with aggression. This effect was partially mediated by aggressive cognitions and empathy. The media violence effect on aggression remained significant even after statistically controlling a number of relevant risk and protective factors (e.g., abusive parenting, peer delinquency), and was similar in magnitude to effects of other risk factors. In support of the cumulative risk model, joint effects of different risk factors on aggressive behavior in each culture were larger than effects of any individual risk factor.}, language = {en} } @article{AlbrechtKopietzLinnetal.2009, author = {Albrecht, Jessica and Kopietz, Rainer and Linn, Jennifer and Sakar, Vehbi and Anzinger, Andrea and Schreder, Tatjana and Pollatos, Olga and Br{\"u}ckmann, Hartmut-Josef and Kobal, Gerd and Wiesmann, Martin}, title = {Activation of olfactory and trigeminal cortical areas following stimulation of the nasal mucosa with low concentrations of S(-)-nicotine vapor : an fMRI study on chemosensory perception}, issn = {1065-9471}, year = {2009}, abstract = {Applied to the nasal mucosa in low concentrations, nicotine vapor evokes odorous sensations (mediated by the olfactory system) whereas at higher concentrations nicotine vapor additionally produces burning and stinging sensations in the nose (mediated by the trigeminal system). The objective of this study was to determine whether intranasal stimulation with suprathreshold concentrations of S(-)-nicotine vapor causes brain activation in olfactory cortical areas or if trigeminal cortical areas are also activated. Individual olfactory detection thresholds for S(-)-nicotine were determined in 19 healthy occasional smokers using a computer-controlled air-dilution olfactometer. Functional magnetic resonance images were acquired using a 1.5T MR scanner with applications of nicotine in concentrations at or just above the individual"s olfactory detection threshold. Subjects reliably perceived the stimuli as being odorous. Accordingly, activation of brain areas known to be involved in processing of olfactory stimuli was identified. Although most of the subjects never or only rarely observed a burning or painful sensation in the nose, brain areas associated with the processing of painful stimuli were activated in all subjects. This indicates that the olfactory and trigeminal systems are activated during perception of nicotine and it is not possible to completely separate olfactory from trigeminal effects by lowering the concentration of the applied nicotine. In conclusion, even at low concentrations that do not consistently lead to painful sensations, intranasally applied nicotine activates both the olfactory and the trigeminal system.}, language = {en} } @article{AlbrechtAnzingerKopietzetal.2008, author = {Albrecht, Jessica and Anzinger, Andrea and Kopietz, Rainer and Sch{\"o}pf, Veronika and Kleemann, Anna Maria and Pollatos, Olga and Wiesmann, Martin}, title = {Test-retest reliability of the olfactory detection threshold test of the Sniffin' Sticks}, issn = {0379-864X}, year = {2008}, abstract = {The aim of the present study was to investigate the test-retest reliability of the olfactory detection threshold subtest of the Sniffin" Sticks test battery, if administered repeatedly on 4 time points. The detection threshold test was repeatedly conducted in 64 healthy subjects. On the first testing session, the threshold test was accomplished 3 times (T(1) = 0 min, T(2) = 35 min, and T(3) = 105 min), representing a short-term testing. A fourth threshold test was conducted on a second testing session (T(4) = 35.1 days after the first testing session), representing a long-term testing. The average scores for olfactory detection threshold for n-butanol did not differ significantly across the 4 points of time. The test-retest reliability (Pearson"s r) between the 4 time points of threshold testing were in a range of 0.43-0.85 (P < 0.01). These results support the notion that the olfactory detection threshold test is a highly reliable method for repeated olfactory testing, even if the test is repeated more than once per day and over a long-term period. It is concluded that the olfactory detection threshold test of the Sniffin" Sticks is suitable for repeated testing during experimental or clinical studies.}, language = {en} } @article{AgtheKayserSchwarzetal.2023, author = {Agthe, Maria and Kayser, Daniela Niesta and Schwarz, Sascha and Maner, Jon K.}, title = {Antecedents of the red-romance effect}, series = {PLOS ONE / Public Library of Science}, volume = {18}, journal = {PLOS ONE / Public Library of Science}, number = {4}, publisher = {PLOS}, address = {San Fransisco}, issn = {1932-6203}, doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0284035}, pages = {19}, year = {2023}, abstract = {The color red has been implicated in a variety of social processes, including those involving mating. While previous research suggests that women sometimes wear red strategically to increase their attractiveness, the replicability of this literature has been questioned. The current research is a reasonably powered conceptual replication designed to strengthen this literature by testing whether women are more inclined to display the color red 1) during fertile (as compared with less fertile) days of the menstrual cycle, and 2) when expecting to interact with an attractive man (as compared with a less attractive man and with a control condition). Analyses controlled for a number of theoretically relevant covariates (relationship status, age, the current weather). Only the latter hypothesis received mixed support (mainly among women on hormonal birth control), whereas results concerning the former hypothesis did not reach significance. Women (N = 281) displayed more red when expecting to interact with an attractive man; findings did not support the prediction that women would increase their display of red on fertile days of the cycle. Findings thus suggested only mixed replicability for the link between the color red and psychological processes involving romantic attraction. They also illustrate the importance of further investigating the boundary conditions of color effects on everyday social processes.}, language = {en} } @phdthesis{Aderhold2004, author = {Aderhold, Detlef E.}, title = {Stabilit{\"a}t und Ver{\"a}nderung von Kindheitserinnerungen / Detlef E. Aderhold}, address = {Potsdam}, pages = {326 S. : graph. Darst.}, year = {2004}, language = {de} } @article{AdamoBaumeisterHohmannetal.2015, author = {Adamo, Nicoletta and Baumeister, Sarah and Hohmann, Sarah and Wolf, Isabella and Holz, Nathalie and Boecker-Schlier, Regina and Laucht, Manfred and Banaschewski, Tobias and Brandeis, Daniel}, title = {Frequency-specific coupling between trial-to-trial fluctuations of neural responses and response-time variability}, series = {Journal of neural transmission}, volume = {122}, journal = {Journal of neural transmission}, number = {8}, publisher = {Springer}, address = {Wien}, issn = {0300-9564}, doi = {10.1007/s00702-015-1382-8}, pages = {1197 -- 1202}, year = {2015}, abstract = {We assessed intra-individual variability of response times (RT) and single-trial P3 amplitudes following targets in healthy adults during a Flanker/NO-GO task. RT variability and variability of the neural responses coupled at the faster frequencies examined (0.07-0.17 Hz) at Pz, the target-P3 maxima, despite non-significant associations for overall variability (standard deviation, SD). Frequency-specific patterns of variability in the single-trial P3 may help to understand the neurophysiology of RT variability and its explanatory models of attention allocation deficits beyond intra-individual variability summary indices such as SD.}, language = {en} } @article{AdamReitenbachPapenmeieretal.2016, author = {Adam, Maurits and Reitenbach, Ivanina and Papenmeier, Frank and Gredeb{\"a}ck, Gustaf and Elsner, Claudia and Elsner, Birgit}, title = {actions, but not for mechanical claws}, series = {Meteoritics \& planetary science : journal of the Meteoritical Society}, volume = {44}, journal = {Meteoritics \& planetary science : journal of the Meteoritical Society}, publisher = {Elsevier}, address = {New York}, issn = {0163-6383}, doi = {10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.05.001}, pages = {29 -- 37}, year = {2016}, abstract = {Previous research indicates that infants' prediction of the goals of observed actions is influenced by own experience with the type of agent performing the action (i.e., human hand vs. non-human agent) as well as by action-relevant features of goal objects (e.g., object size). The present study investigated the combined effects of these factors on 12-month-olds' action prediction. Infants' (N = 49) goal-directed gaze shifts were recorded as they observed 14 trials in which either a human hand or a mechanical claw reached for a small goal area (low-saliency goal) or a large goal area (high-saliency goal). Only infants who had observed the human hand reaching for a high-saliency goal fixated the goal object ahead of time, and they rapidly learned to predict the action goal across trials. By contrast, infants in all other conditions did not track the observed action in a predictive manner, and their gaze shifts to the action goal did not change systematically across trials. Thus, high-saliency goals seem to boost infants' predictive gaze shifts during the observation of human manual actions, but not of actions performed by a mechanical device. This supports the assumption that infants' action predictions are based on interactive effects of action-relevant object features (e.g., size) and own action experience.}, language = {en} } @article{AdamReitenbachElsner2017, author = {Adam, Maurits and Reitenbach, Ivanina and Elsner, Birgit}, title = {Agency cues and 11-month-olds' and adults' anticipation of action goals}, series = {Cognitive Development}, volume = {43}, journal = {Cognitive Development}, publisher = {Elsevier}, address = {New York}, issn = {0885-2014}, doi = {10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.008}, pages = {37 -- 48}, year = {2017}, abstract = {For the processing of goal-directed actions, some accounts emphasize the importance of experience with the action or the agent. Other accounts stress the importance of agency cues. We investigated the impact of agency cues on 11-month-olds' and adults' goal anticipation for a grasping-action performed by a mechanical claw. With an eyetracker, we measured anticipations in two conditions, where the claw was displayed either with or without agency cues. In two experiments, 11-month-olds were predictive when agency cues were present, but reactive when no agency cues were presented. Adults were predictive in both conditions. Furthermore, 11-month-olds rapidly learned to predict the goal in the agency condition, but not in the mechanical condition. Adults' predictions did not change across trials in the agency condition, but decelerated in the mechanical condition. Thus, agency cues and own action experience are important for infants' and adults' online processing of goal-directed actions by non-human agents.}, language = {en} } @article{AdamGumbschButzetal.2021, author = {Adam, Maurits and Gumbsch, Christian and Butz, Martin V. and Elsner, Birgit}, title = {The impact of action effects on infants' predictive gaze shifts for a non-human grasping action at 7, 11, and 18 months}, series = {Frontiers in psychology}, volume = {12}, journal = {Frontiers in psychology}, publisher = {Frontiers Research Foundation}, address = {Lausanne}, issn = {1664-1078}, doi = {10.3389/fpsyg.2021.695550}, pages = {10}, year = {2021}, abstract = {During the observation of goal-directed actions, infants usually predict the goal at an earlier age when the agent is familiar (e.g., human hand) compared to unfamiliar (e.g., mechanical claw). These findings implicate a crucial role of the developing agentive self for infants' processing of others' action goals. Recent theoretical accounts suggest that predictive gaze behavior relies on an interplay between infants' agentive experience (top-down processes) and perceptual information about the agent and the action-event (bottom-up information; e.g., agency cues). The present study examined 7-, 11-, and 18-month-old infants' predictive gaze behavior for a grasping action performed by an unfamiliar tool, depending on infants' age-related action knowledge about tool-use and the display of the agency cue of producing a salient action effect. The results are in line with the notion of a systematic interplay between experience-based top-down processes and cue-based bottom-up information: Regardless of the salient action effect, predictive gaze shifts did not occur in the 7-month-olds (least experienced age group), but did occur in the 18-month-olds (most experienced age group). In the 11-month-olds, however, predictive gaze shifts occurred only when a salient action effect was presented. This sheds new light on how the developing agentive self, in interplay with available agency cues, supports infants' action-goal prediction also for observed tool-use actions.}, language = {en} } @article{AdamElsner2020, author = {Adam, Maurits and Elsner, Birgit}, title = {The impact of salient action effects on 6-, 7-, and 11-month-olds' goal-predictive gaze shifts for a human grasping action}, series = {PLOS ONE}, volume = {15}, journal = {PLOS ONE}, number = {10}, publisher = {Public Library of Science}, address = {San Fransisco}, issn = {1932-6203}, doi = {10.1371/journal.pone.0240165}, pages = {18}, year = {2020}, abstract = {When infants observe a human grasping action, experience-based accounts predict that all infants familiar with grasping actions should be able to predict the goal regardless of additional agency cues such as an action effect. Cue-based accounts, however, suggest that infants use agency cues to identify and predict action goals when the action or the agent is not familiar. From these accounts, we hypothesized that younger infants would need additional agency cues such as a salient action effect to predict the goal of a human grasping action, whereas older infants should be able to predict the goal regardless of agency cues. In three experiments, we presented 6-, 7-, and 11-month-olds with videos of a manual grasping action presented either with or without an additional salient action effect (Exp. 1 and 2), or we presented 7-month-olds with videos of a mechanical claw performing a grasping action presented with a salient action effect (Exp. 3). The 6-month-olds showed tracking gaze behavior, and the 11-month-olds showed predictive gaze behavior, regardless of the action effect. However, the 7-month-olds showed predictive gaze behavior in the action-effect condition, but tracking gaze behavior in the no-action-effect condition and in the action-effect condition with a mechanical claw. The results therefore support the idea that salient action effects are especially important for infants' goal predictions from 7 months on, and that this facilitating influence of action effects is selective for the observation of human hands.}, language = {en} } @article{AdamElsner2018, author = {Adam, Maurits and Elsner, Birgit}, title = {Action effects foster 11-month-olds' prediction of action goals for a non-human agent}, series = {Infant behavior \& development : an international and interdisciplinary journal}, volume = {53}, journal = {Infant behavior \& development : an international and interdisciplinary journal}, publisher = {Elsevier}, address = {New York}, issn = {0163-6383}, doi = {10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.09.002}, pages = {49 -- 55}, year = {2018}, abstract = {Action effects have been stated to be important for infants' processing of goal-directed actions. In this study, 11-month-olds showed equally fast predictive gaze shifts to a claw's action goal when the grasping action was presented either with three agency cues (self-propelled movement, equifinality of goal achievement and a salient action effect) or with only a salient action effect, but infants showed tracking gaze when the claw showed only self-propelled movement and equifinality of goal achievement. The results suggest that action effects, compared to purely kinematic cues, seem to be especially important for infants' online processing of goal-directed actions.}, language = {en} } @phdthesis{Adam2019, author = {Adam, Maurits}, title = {Action-goal predictions in infancy}, school = {Universit{\"a}t Potsdam}, pages = {137}, year = {2019}, language = {en} } @article{AdamBovend'EerdtvanDoorenetal.2012, author = {Adam, Jos J. and Bovend'Eerdt, Thamar J. H. and van Dooren, Fleur E. P. and Fischer, Martin H. and Pratt, Jay}, title = {The closer the better hand proximity dynamically affects letter recognition accuracy}, series = {Attention, perception, \& psychophysics : AP\&P ; a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.}, volume = {74}, journal = {Attention, perception, \& psychophysics : AP\&P ; a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.}, number = {7}, publisher = {Springer}, address = {New York}, issn = {1943-3921}, doi = {10.3758/s13414-012-0339-3}, pages = {1533 -- 1538}, year = {2012}, abstract = {A growing literature has suggested that processing of visual information presented near the hands is facilitated. In this study, we investigated whether the near-hands superiority effect also occurs with the hands moving. In two experiments, participants performed a cyclical bimanual movement task requiring concurrent visual identification of briefly presented letters. For both the static and dynamic hand conditions, the results showed improved letter recognition performance with the hands closer to the stimuli. The finding that the encoding advantage for near-hand stimuli also occurred with the hands moving suggests that the effect is regulated in real time, in accordance with the concept of a bimodal neural system that dynamically updates hand position in external space.}, language = {en} } @incollection{Adam2017, author = {Adam, Hubertus}, title = {Die seelische Belastung von Fl{\"u}chtlingskindern}, series = {Zukunft mit Kindern, Zukunft f{\"u}r Kinder : der UNICEF-Bericht zur Lage der Kinder in Deutschland im europ{\"a}ischen Kontext}, booktitle = {Zukunft mit Kindern, Zukunft f{\"u}r Kinder : der UNICEF-Bericht zur Lage der Kinder in Deutschland im europ{\"a}ischen Kontext}, publisher = {Budrich}, address = {Opladen}, isbn = {978-3-8474-0551-1}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t Potsdam}, pages = {159 -- 174}, year = {2017}, language = {de} } @article{AbujatumAroldKnispeletal.2007, author = {Abujatum, Millaray and Arold, Helga and Knispel, Katharina and Rudolf, Susanne and Schaarschmidt, Uwe}, title = {Intervention durch Training und Beratung}, isbn = {978-3-407-25465-8}, year = {2007}, language = {de} } @article{AbrahamKraheDominicetal.2002, author = {Abraham, Charles and Krah{\´e}, Barbara and Dominic, Robert and Fritsche, Immo}, title = {Do health promotion messages target cognitive and behavioural correlates of condom use? : a content analysis of safer sex promotion leaflets in two countries}, issn = {1359-107X}, year = {2002}, language = {en} } @article{AartsAndersonAndersonetal.2015, author = {Aarts, Alexander A. and Anderson, Joanna E. and Anderson, Christopher J. and Attridge, Peter R. and Attwood, Angela and Axt, Jordan and Babel, Molly and Bahnik, Stepan and Baranski, Erica and Barnett-Cowan, Michael and Bartmess, Elizabeth and Beer, Jennifer and Bell, Raoul and Bentley, Heather and Beyan, Leah and Binion, Grace and Borsboom, Denny and Bosch, Annick and Bosco, Frank A. and Bowman, Sara D. and Brandt, Mark J. and Braswell, Erin and Brohmer, Hilmar and Brown, Benjamin T. and Brown, Kristina and Bruening, Jovita and Calhoun-Sauls, Ann and Callahan, Shannon P. and Chagnon, Elizabeth and Chandler, Jesse and Chartier, Christopher R. and Cheung, Felix and Christopherson, Cody D. and Cillessen, Linda and Clay, Russ and Cleary, Hayley and Cloud, Mark D. and Cohn, Michael and Cohoon, Johanna and Columbus, Simon and Cordes, Andreas and Costantini, Giulio and Alvarez, Leslie D. Cramblet and Cremata, Ed and Crusius, Jan and DeCoster, Jamie and DeGaetano, Michelle A. and Della Penna, Nicolas and den Bezemer, Bobby and Deserno, Marie K. and Devitt, Olivia and Dewitte, Laura and Dobolyi, David G. and Dodson, Geneva T. and Donnellan, M. Brent and Donohue, Ryan and Dore, Rebecca A. and Dorrough, Angela and Dreber, Anna and Dugas, Michelle and Dunn, Elizabeth W. and Easey, Kayleigh and Eboigbe, Sylvia and Eggleston, Casey and Embley, Jo and Epskamp, Sacha and Errington, Timothy M. and Estel, Vivien and Farach, Frank J. and Feather, Jenelle and Fedor, Anna and Fernandez-Castilla, Belen and Fiedler, Susann and Field, James G. and Fitneva, Stanka A. and Flagan, Taru and Forest, Amanda L. and Forsell, Eskil and Foster, Joshua D. and Frank, Michael C. and Frazier, Rebecca S. and Fuchs, Heather and Gable, Philip and Galak, Jeff and Galliani, Elisa Maria and Gampa, Anup and Garcia, Sara and Gazarian, Douglas and Gilbert, Elizabeth and Giner-Sorolla, Roger and Gl{\"o}ckner, Andreas and G{\"o}llner, Lars and Goh, Jin X. and Goldberg, Rebecca and Goodbourn, Patrick T. and Gordon-McKeon, Shauna and Gorges, Bryan and Gorges, Jessie and Goss, Justin and Graham, Jesse and Grange, James A. and Gray, Jeremy and Hartgerink, Chris and Hartshorne, Joshua and Hasselman, Fred and Hayes, Timothy and Heikensten, Emma and Henninger, Felix and Hodsoll, John and Holubar, Taylor and Hoogendoorn, Gea and Humphries, Denise J. and Hung, Cathy O. -Y. and Immelman, Nathali and Irsik, Vanessa C. and Jahn, Georg and Jaekel, Frank and Jekel, Marc and Johannesson, Magnus and Johnson, Larissa G. and Johnson, David J. and Johnson, Kate M. and Johnston, William J. and Jonas, Kai and Joy-Gaba, Jennifer A. and Kappes, Heather Barry and Kelso, Kim and Kidwell, Mallory C. and Kim, Seung Kyung and Kirkhart, Matthew and Kleinberg, Bennett and Knezevic, Goran and Kolorz, Franziska Maria and Kossakowski, Jolanda J. and Krause, Robert Wilhelm and Krijnen, Job and Kuhlmann, Tim and Kunkels, Yoram K. and Kyc, Megan M. and Lai, Calvin K. and Laique, Aamir and Lakens, Daniel and Lane, Kristin A. and Lassetter, Bethany and Lazarevic, Ljiljana B. and LeBel, Etienne P. and Lee, Key Jung and Lee, Minha and Lemm, Kristi and Levitan, Carmel A. and Lewis, Melissa and Lin, Lin and Lin, Stephanie and Lippold, Matthias and Loureiro, Darren and Luteijn, Ilse and Mackinnon, Sean and Mainard, Heather N. and Marigold, Denise C. and Martin, Daniel P. and Martinez, Tylar and Masicampo, E. J. and Matacotta, Josh and Mathur, Maya and May, Michael and Mechin, Nicole and Mehta, Pranjal and Meixner, Johannes and Melinger, Alissa and Miller, Jeremy K. and Miller, Mallorie and Moore, Katherine and M{\"o}schl, Marcus and Motyl, Matt and M{\"u}ller, Stephanie M. and Munafo, Marcus and Neijenhuijs, Koen I. and Nervi, Taylor and Nicolas, Gandalf and Nilsonne, Gustav and Nosek, Brian A. and Nuijten, Michele B. and Olsson, Catherine and Osborne, Colleen and Ostkamp, Lutz and Pavel, Misha and Penton-Voak, Ian S. and Perna, Olivia and Pernet, Cyril and Perugini, Marco and Pipitone, R. Nathan and Pitts, Michael and Plessow, Franziska and Prenoveau, Jason M. and Rahal, Rima-Maria and Ratliff, Kate A. and Reinhard, David and Renkewitz, Frank and Ricker, Ashley A. and Rigney, Anastasia and Rivers, Andrew M. and Roebke, Mark and Rutchick, Abraham M. and Ryan, Robert S. and Sahin, Onur and Saide, Anondah and Sandstrom, Gillian M. and Santos, David and Saxe, Rebecca and Schlegelmilch, Rene and Schmidt, Kathleen and Scholz, Sabine and Seibel, Larissa and Selterman, Dylan Faulkner and Shaki, Samuel and Simpson, William B. and Sinclair, H. Colleen and Skorinko, Jeanine L. M. and Slowik, Agnieszka and Snyder, Joel S. and Soderberg, Courtney and Sonnleitner, Carina and Spencer, Nick and Spies, Jeffrey R. and Steegen, Sara and Stieger, Stefan and Strohminger, Nina and Sullivan, Gavin B. and Talhelm, Thomas and Tapia, Megan and te Dorsthorst, Anniek and Thomae, Manuela and Thomas, Sarah L. and Tio, Pia and Traets, Frits and Tsang, Steve and Tuerlinckx, Francis and Turchan, Paul and Valasek, Milan and Van Aert, Robbie and van Assen, Marcel and van Bork, Riet and van de Ven, Mathijs and van den Bergh, Don and van der Hulst, Marije and van Dooren, Roel and van Doorn, Johnny and van Renswoude, Daan R. and van Rijn, Hedderik and Vanpaemel, Wolf and Echeverria, Alejandro Vasquez and Vazquez, Melissa and Velez, Natalia and Vermue, Marieke and Verschoor, Mark and Vianello, Michelangelo and Voracek, Martin and Vuu, Gina and Wagenmakers, Eric-Jan and Weerdmeester, Joanneke and Welsh, Ashlee and Westgate, Erin C. and Wissink, Joeri and Wood, Michael and Woods, Andy and Wright, Emily and Wu, Sining and Zeelenberg, Marcel and Zuni, Kellylynn}, title = {Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science}, series = {Science}, volume = {349}, journal = {Science}, number = {6251}, publisher = {American Assoc. for the Advancement of Science}, address = {Washington}, organization = {Open Sci Collaboration}, issn = {1095-9203}, doi = {10.1126/science.aac4716}, pages = {8}, year = {2015}, abstract = {Reproducibility is a defining feature of science, but the extent to which it characterizes current research is unknown. We conducted replications of 100 experimental and correlational studies published in three psychology journals using high-powered designs and original materials when available. Replication effects were half the magnitude of original effects, representing a substantial decline. Ninety-seven percent of original studies had statistically significant results. Thirty-six percent of replications had statistically significant results; 47\% of original effect sizes were in the 95\% confidence interval of the replication effect size; 39\% of effects were subjectively rated to have replicated the original result; and if no bias in original results is assumed, combining original and replication results left 68\% with statistically significant effects. Correlational tests suggest that replication success was better predicted by the strength of original evidence than by characteristics of the original and replication teams.}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-11022, title = {Psychologie der Arbeitssicherheit und Gesundheit : Arbeitsschutz, Gesundheit und Wirtschaftlichkeit ; 14. Workshop 2007}, editor = {B{\"a}renz, Peter and Metz, Anna-Marie and Rothe, Heinz-J{\"u}rgen}, publisher = {Asanger}, address = {Kr{\"o}ning}, isbn = {978-3-89334-476-5}, pages = {478 S.}, year = {2007}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-11073, title = {The 3rd International IEEE Scientific Conference on Physics and Control (PhysCon 2007) : September 3rd-7th 2007 at the University of Potsdam}, editor = {Kurths, J{\"u}rgen and Fradkov, Alexander and Chen, Guanrong}, publisher = {Univ.-Verl. Potsdam}, address = {Potsdam}, isbn = {978-3-939469-89-6}, pages = {345 S.}, year = {2007}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-11041, title = {Ger{\"u}stet f{\"u}r den Schulalltag : Psychologische Unterst{\"u}tzungsangebote f{\"u}r Lehrerinnen und Lehrer}, editor = {Schaarschmidt, Uwe and Kieschke, Ulf}, publisher = {Beltz}, address = {Weinheim}, isbn = {978-3-407-25465-8}, pages = {252 S.}, year = {2007}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-12948, title = {Halbtagsjobber? : psychische Gesundheit im Lehrberuf - Analyse eines ver{\"a}nderungsbed{\"u}rftigen Zustandes}, editor = {Schaarschmidt, Uwe}, publisher = {Beltz}, address = {Weinheim}, isbn = {3-407-25407-5}, pages = {172 S.}, year = {2005}, language = {de} } @article{OPUS4-12593, title = {Die erweiterte Vorsorgeuntersuchung (EVU) : eine entwicklungsneurologische Erg{\"a}nzung zur p{\"a}diatrischen Vorsorge bei U4 bis U9}, year = {2006}, abstract = {Die Arbeitsgruppe berichtete an dieser Stelle bereits fr{\"u}her {\"u}ber die ersten Ergebnisse ihrer Arbeit zur entwicklungsneurologischen und - psychologischen Erg{\"a}nzung der Vorsorgeuntersuchung f{\"u}r U 4 bis U 9 (Melchers et al. 2000). Da dieser Werkstattbericht große Resonanz ausl{\"o}ste, soll hier {\"u}ber das Ergebnis der Arbeiten und die ersten Erfahrungen zur Anwendung in der Praxis berichtet werden. Zwischenzeitlich konnte die Normierung auf der Grundlage einer Feldstichprobe von insgesamt 1743 Kindern aus dem gesamten Bundesgebiet abgeschlossen werden. In Verbindung mit der Standardisierung von Untersuchungsmaterial und Durchf{\"u}hrungsanweisungen sowie Auswertungs- und Interpretationsrichtlinien steht f{\"u}r die kinder{\"a}rztliche Praxis jetzt ein Screeninstrument zur Verf{\"u}gung, mit dessen Hilfe die Erfassung und Beurteilung von drohenden und manifesten Retardierungen m{\"o}glich ist. Im Folgenden werden die Ergebnisse der Gesamtstudie auszugsweise dargestellt.}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-16030, title = {Interventionen bei Lernst{\"o}rungen : F{\"o}rderung, Training und Therapie in der Praxis}, editor = {Lauth, Gerhard W. and Gr{\"u}nke, Matthias and Brunstein, Joachim Clemens}, publisher = {Hogrefe Verl. f{\"u}r Psychologie}, address = {G{\"o}ttingen}, isbn = {3-8017-1655-4}, pages = {475 S.}, year = {2003}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-16062, title = {Lehrbuch der Klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters}, series = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, journal = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, editor = {Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {THIEME}, address = {Stuttgart}, isbn = {3-13-126082-3}, pages = {586 S.}, year = {2003}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-16192, title = {Stimmungsprobleme bew{\"a}ltigen : Arbeitsbuch f{\"u}r Kursteilnehmer}, series = {Materialie / Deutsche Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Verhaltenstheraphie}, volume = {55}, journal = {Materialie / Deutsche Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Verhaltenstheraphie}, editor = {Ihle, Wolfgang and Herrle, Johannes}, publisher = {DGVT Deutsche Gesellschaft f. Verhaltenstherapie}, address = {T{\"u}bingen}, isbn = {3-87159-355-9}, pages = {117 S.}, year = {2003}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-16377, title = {Stimmungsprobleme bew{\"a}ltigen : Manual f{\"u}r Kursleiter}, series = {Materialie / Deutsche Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Verhaltenstheraphie}, volume = {54}, journal = {Materialie / Deutsche Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Verhaltenstheraphie}, editor = {Ihle, Wolfgang and Herrle, Johannes}, publisher = {DGVT-Verl.}, address = {T{\"u}bingen}, isbn = {3-87159-354-0}, pages = {196 S.}, year = {2003}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-17076, title = {Lehrbuch der Klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters}, series = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, journal = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, editor = {Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {THIEME}, address = {Stuttgart}, isbn = {3-13-126081-5}, pages = {586 S.}, year = {2002}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-721, title = {Interdisciplinary studies on information structure : ISIS ; working papers of the SFB 632. - Vol. 1}, editor = {Ishihara, Shinichiro and Schmitz, Michaela and Schwarz, Anne}, issn = {1866-4725}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-8237}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t Potsdam}, year = {2004}, abstract = {Contents: A1: Phonology and syntax of focussing and topicalisation: Gisbert Fanselow: Cyclic Phonology-Syntax-Interaction: Movement to First Position in German Caroline F{\´e}ry and Laura Herbst: German Sentence Accent Revisited Shinichiro Ishihara: Prosody by Phase: Evidence from Focus Intonation-Wh-scope Correspondence in Japanese A2: Quantification and information structure: Cornelia Endriss and Stefan Hinterwimmer: The Influence of Tense in Adverbial Quantification A3: Rhetorical Structure in Spoken Language: Modeling of Global Prosodic Parameters: Ekaterina Jasinskaja, J{\"o}rg Mayer and David Schlangen: Discourse Structure and Information Structure: Interfaces and Prosodic Realization B2: Focussing in African Tchadic languages: Katharina Hartmann and Malte Zimmermann: Focus Strategies in Chadic: The Case of Tangale Revisited D1: Linguistic database for information structure: Annotation and retrieval: Stefanie Dipper, Michael G{\"o}tze, Manfred Stede and Tillmann Wegst: ANNIS: A Linguistic Database for Exploring Information Structure}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-723, title = {Heterogeneity in focus : creating and using linguistic databases}, editor = {Dipper, Stefanie and G{\"o}tze, Michael and Stede, Manfred}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}tsverlag Potsdam}, address = {Potsdam}, isbn = {978-3-937786-48-3}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-8244}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t Potsdam}, pages = {145}, year = {2005}, abstract = {The papers in this volume were presented at the workshop Heterogeneity in Linguistic Databases', which took place on July 9, 2004 at the University of Potsdam. The workshop was organized by project D1: Linguistic Database for Information Structure: Annotation and Retrieval', a member project of the SFB 632, a collaborative research center entitled Information Structure: the Linguistic Means for Structuring Utterances, Sentences and Texts'. The workshop brought together both developers and users of linguistic databases from a number of research projects which work on an empirical basis, all of which have to cope with different sorts of heterogeneity: primary linguistic data and annotated information may be heterogeneous, as well as the data structures representing them. The first four papers (by Wagner, Schmidt, L{\"u}deling, and Witt) address aspects of heterogeneous data from the point of view of database developers; the remaining three papers (by Meyer, Smith, and Teich/Fankhauser) focus on data exploitation by the users.}, language = {en} } @inproceedings{OPUS4-1658, title = {Seelisch gesund von Anfang an : Programm und Abstracts des 26. Symposiums der Fachgruppe Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie der Deutschen Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Psychologie, 1. - 3. Mai 2008 in Potsdam}, editor = {Warschburger, Petra and Ihle, Wolfgang and Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}tsverlag Potsdam}, address = {Potsdam}, isbn = {978-3-940793-34-8}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-17551}, pages = {302}, year = {2008}, abstract = {Der Tagungsband enth{\"a}lt das Programm und die Abstracts des 26. Symposiums der Fachgruppe Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie der Deutschen Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Psychologie, veranstaltet an der Universit{\"a}t Potsdam vom 1. bis 3. Mai 2008. Etwa 450 Kongressteilnehmer pr{\"a}sentieren den aktuellen Forschungs- und Wissensstand der Klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie in Deutschland. Grußworte halten die brandenburgische Ministerin f{\"u}r Arbeit, Soziales, Gesundheit und Familie, Dagmar Ziegler, die Pr{\"a}sidentin der Universit{\"a}t Potsdam, Prof. Dr.-Ing. Dr. Sabine Kunst, sowie Prof. Dr. Michael Linden als Vertreter der Deutschen Gesellschaft f{\"u}r Psychiatrie, Psychotherapie und Nervenheilkunde (DGPPN). Zu den Themenschwerpunkten des Kongresses geh{\"o}ren Einflussfaktoren auf die psychische Gesundheit {\"A}lterer, Impulsivit{\"a}t, Schlaf- und Traumforschung in der Klinischen Psychologie, Behandlung von Essst{\"o}rungen, Wirksamkeitsstudien psychischer St{\"o}rungen des Kindes- und Jugendalters, Angst und Depression, Behandlung von Kriegs- und Folteropfern, Risiko- und Schutzfaktoren der Kindesentwicklung sowie Adipositas im Kindes- und Jugendalter. Außer den Vortr{\"a}gen gibt es eine Pr{\"a}sentation von etwa 150 Postern. Zum Programm der Tagung geh{\"o}rt ebenso die Verleihung des Klaus-Grawe-Awards for the Advancement of Innovative Research in Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy an Prof. Dr. Timothy J. Strauman von der Duke University (USA), die Verleihung der Nachwuchswissenschaftler- und Posterpreise sowie ein Pre-conference Workshop f{\"u}r Doktorandinnen und Doktoranden der Klinischen Psychologie zum Thema "Verhaltens- und Molekulargenetik".}, subject = {Konferenz}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-1195, title = {Interdisciplinary studies on information structure : ISIS ; Working papers of the SFB 632 - Vol. 5}, editor = {Ishihara, Shinichiro and Schmitz, Michaela and Schwarz, Anne}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}tsverlag Potsdam}, address = {Potsdam}, issn = {1866-4725}, url = {http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus-13047}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t Potsdam}, pages = {221}, year = {2006}, abstract = {In this paper we compare the behaviour of adverbs of frequency (de Swart 1993) like usually with the behaviour of adverbs of quantity like for the most part in sentences that contain plural definites. We show that sentences containing the former type of Q-adverb evidence that Quantificational Variability Effects (Berman 1991) come about as an indirect effect of quantification over situations: in order for quantificational variability readings to arise, these sentences have to obey two newly observed constraints that clearly set them apart from sentences containing corresponding quantificational DPs, and that can plausibly be explained under the assumption that quantification over (the atomic parts of) complex situations is involved. Concerning sentences with the latter type of Q-adverb, on the other hand, such evidence is lacking: with respect to the constraints just mentioned, they behave like sentences that contain corresponding quantificational DPs. We take this as evidence that Q-adverbs like for the most part do not quantify over the atomic parts of sum eventualities in the cases under discussion (as claimed by Nakanishi and Romero (2004)), but rather over the atomic parts of the respective sum individuals.}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-47977, title = {Minor cosmopolitan}, editor = {Xiang, Zairong}, publisher = {Diaphanes}, address = {Berlin}, isbn = {978-3-0358-0304-4}, pages = {256}, year = {2020}, abstract = {Cosmopolitanism is a theory about how to live together. The earliest formulation of cosmopolitanism in the West could be dated to as early as the fourth century BCE in ancient Greece by Diogenes, who famously said that he was a "citizen of the world - kosmopolit{\^e}s," an idea later picked up by Immanuel Kant, the German philosopher who proposed a philosophy of a world of "perpetual peace." When cosmopolitanism first emerged as a political idea for modernity in the European Enlightenment, the project embraced the liberal promises of a globalizing economy, yet remained oblivious to, and even complicit with, capitalism, slavery and colonialism. It centered on the male, bourgeois, and white liberal subject, irrespective of the ongoing disenfranchisement, dehumanization, and extermination of its Others. At the dawn of the 21st century, and in the wake of rapid globalization however, academics, politicians and other pundits enthusiastically declared cosmopolitanism to be no longer just a philosophical ideal, but a real, existing fact. Across the globe, they argued, people were increasingly thinking and feeling beyond the nation, considering themselves citizens of the world. Meanwhile, the global ecological crisis worsens, fascism with different outfits returns in many places of the world, the repression of women, sexual, racial, class and other minorities on a global scale persists; the so called "refugee crisis" inundates the mediascape and political spectacle. Not much of those cosmopolitan promises have left it seems. Perhaps precisely because of this, however, it seems to be an absolute necessity for scholars, activists, and artists today to face the complexities and promises cosmopolitanism has raised although not adequately answered. What has happened to the cosmopolitan promise, and who betrayed it? "Minor cosmopolitanisms" wishes to challenge the underlying premises of 'major' cosmopolitanism without letting go of the unfulfilled emancipatory potential of the concept at large. It wants to rethink cosmopolitanisms in the plural, and trace multiple origins and trajectories of cosmopolitan thought from across the globe. Regarding cosmopolitanisms as emerging through diverse locally, historically and politically specific practices, minor cosmopolitanisms are predicated on difference without abandoning the quest for a shared vision of conviviality and justice. It seeks to answer: how to live at once with our difference and shared struggle? How to think our complicity with even those we most resist? Who sustains the world's flourishing despite all this?}, language = {en} } @article{OPUS4-26750, title = {Untersuchungen zu Problemerleben und -bew{\"a}ltigung Potsdamer Sch{\"u}ler : Forschungsbericht}, editor = {Kirsch, B{\"a}rbel}, publisher = {Univ.}, address = {Potsdam}, pages = {124 S.}, year = {1995}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-27648, title = {Entwicklung und Einsatz wissensorientierter Unterst{\"u}tzungssysteme : Ergebnisse aus dem Verbundvorhaben WEDA}, series = {Werkstattberichte aus Wissenschaft und Technik}, volume = {11}, journal = {Werkstattberichte aus Wissenschaft und Technik}, editor = {Hacker, Winfried and Rothe, Heinz-J{\"u}rgen and Wandke, Hartmut and Ziegler, J{\"u}rgen}, publisher = {Wirtschaftsverl. NW Verl. f{\"u}r Neue Wiss}, address = {Bremerhaven}, isbn = {3-89429-968-1}, pages = {273 S.}, year = {1995}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-34460, title = {Lehrbuch der Klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie des Kindes- und Jugendalters}, editor = {Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {THIEME}, address = {Stuttgart}, isbn = {978-3-13-126084-0}, pages = {672 S.}, year = {2011}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-29586, title = {Beratungspsychologie}, editor = {Warschburger, Petra}, publisher = {Springer-Verlag}, address = {Heidelberg}, isbn = {978-3-540-79060-0}, pages = {XI, 277 S.}, year = {2009}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-28799, title = {Psychologie in {\"O}sterreich}, editor = {Schaarschmidt, Uwe}, publisher = {WUV-Verl.}, address = {Wien}, year = {1993}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-30509, title = {Lehrbuch der Klinischen Psychologie und Psychotherapie bei Kindern und Jugendlichen : 90 Tabellen}, series = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, journal = {Klinische Psychologie und Psychiatrie}, editor = {Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {THIEME}, address = {Stuttgart [u.a.]}, isbn = {978-3-13-126083-3}, pages = {XIX, 638 S. : Ill., graph. Darst.}, year = {2008}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-25307, title = {Early childhood intervention : theory, evaluation and practice}, series = {Prevention and intervention in childhood and adolescence}, volume = {20}, journal = {Prevention and intervention in childhood and adolescence}, editor = {Brambring, Michael and Rauh, Hellgard}, publisher = {de Gruyter}, address = {Berlin ; New York}, pages = {513 S.}, year = {1996}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-22087, title = {Handbuch betriebliche Gesundheitsf{\"o}rderung : Arbeitspsychologische und organisationspsychologische Methoden und Konzepte}, editor = {Bamberg, Eva and Ducki, Antje and Metz, Anna-Marie}, publisher = {Verl. f{\"u}r Angewandte Psychologie}, address = {G{\"o}ttingen}, pages = {534 S.}, year = {1998}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-26311, title = {Psychologische Erkenntnisse und Methoden als Grundlage f{\"u}r die Gestaltung von Mensch-Maschine-Systemen}, series = {Forschungsbericht / Zentrum Mensch-Maschine-Systeme}, volume = {96, 3}, journal = {Forschungsbericht / Zentrum Mensch-Maschine-Systeme}, editor = {Rothe, Heinz-J{\"u}rgen and Kolrep, Harald}, publisher = {ZMMS}, address = {Berlin}, pages = {55 S.}, year = {1996}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-17227, title = {Diagnostik von Motivation und Selbstkonzept}, series = {Tests und Trends : Jahrbuch der p{\"a}dagogisch-psychologischen Diagnostik N.F.}, volume = {2}, journal = {Tests und Trends : Jahrbuch der p{\"a}dagogisch-psychologischen Diagnostik N.F.}, editor = {Stiensmeier-Pelster, Joachim and Rheinberg, Falko}, publisher = {Hogrefe}, address = {G{\"o}ttingen}, isbn = {3-8017-1674-0}, pages = {303 S.}, year = {2002}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-17414, title = {Aspekte der Leistungsmotivation : Lehr- und Lernmaterial f{\"u}r Lehrer, Lehramts- und Psychologiestudenten und interessierte Eltern}, editor = {Lund, Brigitte}, publisher = {Univ}, address = {Potsdam}, isbn = {3-935024-49-5}, pages = {1 CD-ROM : farb., mit Ton ; 12 cm}, year = {2002}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-19133, title = {Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht [Themendoppelheft: Bindung]}, editor = {Rauh, Hellgard}, year = {2000}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-19265, title = {Zeitschrift f{\"u}r Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie : Themenheft Entwicklungspsychopathologie ; Ergebnisse aus Langzeitstudien}, editor = {Esser, G{\"u}nter}, publisher = {Hogrefe}, address = {G{\"o}ttingen}, issn = {0084-5345}, year = {2000}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-19328, title = {Iznennye problemy i sposoby ich razreschenija schkol'nikami 12 - 18 let : (krosskul'turnoe sravnenie Potsdam - Sankt-Peterburg)}, editor = {Kirsch, B{\"a}rbel and Regusch, Ludmilla A. and Alekseeva, Elena V.}, publisher = {Rossijskij gosudarstvennyj pedagogiQQQA7eskij universitet im. A. I. Gercena}, address = {Sankt Peterburg}, isbn = {5-8064-0313-0}, year = {2000}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-20429, title = {Beratung im Wandel : eine Ver{\"o}ffentlichung der Bundeskonferenz f{\"u}r Erziehungsberatung (bke)}, series = {Edition Psychologie und P{\"a}dagogik}, journal = {Edition Psychologie und P{\"a}dagogik}, editor = {Marschner, Lutz}, publisher = {Matthias-Gr{\"u}newald-Verl.}, address = {Mainz}, isbn = {3-7867-2180-7}, pages = {131 S.}, year = {1999}, language = {de} } @book{OPUS4-28046, title = {Aging and Cognition : a journal on normal and dysfunctional development}, editor = {Kliegl, Reinhold}, publisher = {Swets \& Zeitlinger}, address = {Lisse}, year = {1994}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-18659, title = {Ageing and executive control}, series = {European Journal of Cognitive Psychology : Special Issues}, volume = {13}, journal = {European Journal of Cognitive Psychology : Special Issues}, editor = {Mayr, Ulrich and Spieler, D. and Kliegl, Reinhold}, publisher = {Psychology Press}, address = {Hove}, isbn = {1-84169-908-x}, pages = {303 S.}, year = {2001}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-22057, title = {Color vision : perspectives from different disciplines}, editor = {Backhaus, Werner G. K. and Kliegl, Reinhold and Werner, John S.}, publisher = {de Gruyter}, address = {Berlin}, isbn = {3-11-015431-5}, pages = {XV, 343 S. : Ill.}, year = {1998}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-29152, title = {Psychology and Aging : published quarterly by the American Psychological Association}, editor = {Kliegl, Reinhold}, publisher = {Ass.}, address = {Arlington, Va}, year = {1992}, language = {en} } @book{OPUS4-34329, title = {Conference Abstracts : ECEM 2007 ; 14. European Conference on Eye Movements ECEM2007, Potsdam, August 19 - 23, 2007}, series = {Journal of eye movement research : Special issue}, volume = {1}, journal = {Journal of eye movement research : Special issue}, editor = {Kliegl, Reinhold}, publisher = {Universit{\"a}t}, address = {Potsdam}, issn = {1995-8692}, pages = {179 S.}, year = {2007}, language = {en} }